248
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FUTURE TIME PERSPECTIVE AND MEMORY CONTROL BELIEFS IN OLDER ADULTS

&

References

  • Agrigoroaei, S., Neupert, S. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2013). Maintaining a sense of control in the context of cognitive challenge: Greater stability in control beliefs benefits working memory. The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(1), 49–59.
  • Allen, J. Y., Hilgeman, M. M., & Allen, R. S. (2011). Prospective end-of-life treatment decisions and perceived vulnerability: Future time left to live and memory self-efficacy. Aging & Mental Health, 15(1), 122–131. doi:10.1080/13607863.2010.505229
  • Anderson, J. C., & Berbeing, D. W. (1984). The effect of sampling error on convergence, improper solutions, and goodness-of-fit indices for maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis. Psychometrika, 49, 155–173. doi:10.1007/BF02294170
  • Andre, L., van Vianen, A. E. M., Peetsma, T. T. D., & Oort, F. J. (2018). Motivational power of future time perspective: Meta-analyses in education, work, and health. PLoS One, 13(1), e0190492. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0190492
  • Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educational Psychologist, 28, 117–148. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3
  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman and Company.
  • Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 164–180. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00011.x
  • Barber, S. J., & Tan, S. T. (2018). Ageism affects older adults’ future time perspective. GeroPsych, 31(3), 115–126. doi:10.1024/1662-9647/a000189
  • Borkowski, J. G., Carr, M., Rellinger, E., & Pressley, M. (1990). Self-regulated cognition: Interdependence of metacognition, attributions, and self-esteem. In B. F. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (Vol. 1, pp. 53–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (1994). Self-percepts of control in middle and later adulthood: Buffering losses by rescaling goals. Psychology and Aging, 9(2), 275–283. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.9.2.265
  • Brothers, A., Gabrian, M., Wahl, H., & Diehl, M. (2016). Future time perspective and awareness of age-related change: Examining their role in predicting psychological well-being. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 605–617. doi:10.1037/pag0000101
  • Brothers, A., Chui, H., Diehl, M., & Pruchno, R. (2014). Measuring future time perspective across adulthood: Development and evaluation of a brief multidimensional questionnaire. The Gerontologist, 54(6), 1075–1088. doi:10.1093/geront/gnu076
  • Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312(5782), 1913–1915. doi:10.1126/science.1127488
  • Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., & Charles, S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 103–123. doi:10.1023/A:1024569803230
  • Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–181.
  • Carstensen, L. L., & Lang, F. R. (1996). Future time perspective scale (Unpublished manuscript). Stanford University.
  • Cate, R. A., & John, O. P. (2007). Testing models of the structure and development of future time perspective: Maintaining a focus on opportunities in middle age. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 186–201. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.186
  • Cottle, T. J. (1976). Perceiving time: A psychological investigation with men and women. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Dark-Freudeman, A., West, R. L., & Viverito, K. M. (2006). Future selves and aging: Older adults’ memory fears. Educational Gerontology, 32(2), 85–109. doi:10.1080/03601270500388125
  • Devolder, P. A., & Pressley, M. (1992). Causal attributions and strategy use in relation to memory performance differences in younger and older adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6(7), 629–642. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0720
  • Dixon, R. A., & Hultsch, D. F. (1983). Structure and development of metamemory in adulthood. Journal of Gerontology, 38(6), 682–688.
  • Elliott, E., & Lachman, M. E. (1989). Enhancing memory by modifying control beliefs, attributions, and performance goals in the elderly. In P. S. Fry (Ed.), Advances in psychology, 57. Psychological perspectives of helplessness and control in the elderly (pp. 339–367). Oxford, UK: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)60986-3
  • Fingerman, K. L., & Perlmutter, M. (1995). Future time perspective and life events across adulthood. The Journal of General Psychology, 122(1), 95–111. doi:10.1080/00221309.1995.9921225
  • Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lutz, A. M. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development. Psychology and Aging, 14(4), 595–604.
  • Grühn, D., Sharifian, N., & Chu, Q. (2016). The limits of a limited future time perspective in explaining age differences in emotional functioning. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 583–593. doi:10.1037/pag0000060
  • Hastings, E. C., & West, R. L. (2011). Goal orientation and self-efficacy in relation to memory in adulthood. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18(4), 471–493. doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.575926
  • Hertzog, C., & Hultsch, D. F. (2000). Metacognition in adulthood and aging. In T. Salthouse & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 417–466). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Horhota, M., Lineweaver, T., Ositelu, M., Summers, K., & Herzog, C. (2012). Young and older adults’ beliefs about effective ways to mitigate age-related memory decline. Psychology and Aging, 27(2), 293–304. doi:10.1037/a0026088
  • Hutchens, R. L., Kinsella, G. J., Ong, B., Pike, K. E., Clare, L., Ames, D., … Parsons, S. (2013). Relationship between control beliefs, strategy use, and memory performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68(6), 862–871. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt016
  • Jacobs, J. M., Waddell, E. L., & Webb, A. K. (2011). Predictors of health locus of control in older adults. Current Psychology, 30, 173. doi:10.1007/s12144-011-9108-z
  • Jones, K. M., Whitbourne, S. K., Whitbourne, S. B., & Skultety, K. M. (2009). Identity processes and memory controllability in middle and later adulthood. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 28(5), 582–599. doi:10.1177/0733464808330823
  • Klineberg, S. L. (1967). Changes in outlook on the future between childhood and adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 (2, Pt. 1), 185–193. doi:10.1037/h0024988
  • Kobayashi, L. C., Beekman, R. J., & Meisel, S. F. (2017). Biopsychosocial predictors of perceived life expectancy in a national sample of older men and women. PLoS One, 12(12), e0189245. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0189245
  • Kooij, D. T., de Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G., & Dikkers, J. S. (2013). Beyond chronological age. Examining perceived future time and subjective health as age-related mediators in relation to work-related motivations and well-being. Work & Stress, 27(1), 88–105. doi:10.1080/02678373.2013.769328
  • Kooij, D. T. A. M., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., & Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(8), 867–893. doi:10.1037/apl0000306
  • Kotter-Grühn, D., & Smith, J. (2011). When time is running out: Changes in positive future perception and their relationships to change sin well-being in old age. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 381–387. doi:10.1037/a0022223
  • Kozik, P., Drewelies, J., Duzel, S., Demuth, I., Steinhagen-Thiessen, E., Gerstorf, D., & Hoppmann, C. A. (2019). Future time perspective: Dimensions of opportunities, life, and time are differentially associated with physical health, cognitive functioning, and well-being in old age. Aging & Mental Health. doi:10.1080/13607863.2019.1594159
  • Kozik, P., Hoppmann, C. A., & Gerstorf, D. (2015). Future time perspective: Opportunities and limitations are differentially associated with subjective well-being and hair cortisol concentration. Gerontology, 61(2), 166–174. doi:10.1159/000368716
  • Kuppelweiser, V. G., & Sarstedt, M. (2014). Applying the future time perspective scale to advertising research. International Journal of Advertising, 33(1), 113–136. doi:10.2501/IJA-33-1-113-136
  • Lachman, M. E. (1986). Locus of control in aging research: A case for multidimensional and domain-specific assessment. Psychology and Aging, 1(1), 34–40.
  • Lachman, M. E. (1991). Perceived control over memory aging: Developmental and intervention perspectives. Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 159–175. doi:10.1111/josi.1991.47.issue-4
  • Lachman, M. E. (2006). Perceived control over aging-related declines: Adaptive beliefs and behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 282–286. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00453.x
  • Lachman, M. E., Neupert, S. D., & Agrigoroaei, S. (2011). The relevance of control beliefs for health and aging. In K. W. Schaie & S. L. Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (7th ed., pp. 175–190). New York, NY: Elsevier.
  • Lachman, M. E., & Agrigoroaei, S. (2012). Low perceived control as a risk factor for episodic memory: The mediational role of anxiety and task interference. Memory & Cognition, 40(2), 287–296. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0140-x
  • Lachman, M. E., Bandura, M., Weaver, S. L., & Elliott, E. (1995). Assessing memory control beliefs: The memory controllability inventory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 2(1), 67–84. doi:10.1080/13825589508256589
  • Lachman, M. E., Steinberg, E. S., & Trotter, S. D. (1987). Effects of control beliefs and attributions on memory self-assessments and performance. Psychology and Aging, 2(3), 266–271.
  • Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). Sociodemographic variations in the sense of control by domain: Findings from the McArthur studies of midlife. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 553–562.
  • Lachman, M. E., Weaver, S. L., Bandura, M., Elliott, E., & Lewkowitz, C. J. (1992). Improving memory and control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self-generated strategies. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological and Social Sciences, 27(5), 293–299.
  • Lang, F. R. (2000). Endings and continuity of social relationships: Maximizing intrinsic benefits within personal networks when feeling near to death. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17(2), 155–182. doi: 10.1177/0265407500172001
  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 125–139. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125
  • Laureiro-Martinez, D., Trujillo, C. A., & Unda, J. (2017). Time perspective and age: A review of age associated differences. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 101. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00101
  • Lewin, K. (1939). Field theory and experimentin social psychology: Concepts and methods. The American Journal of Sociology, 44(6), 868–897.
  • Liao, H. -W., & Carstensen, L. L. (2018). Future time perspective: Time horizons and beyond. GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 31, 163–167. doi: 10.1024/1662-9647/a000194
  • Lineweaver, T. T., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Adults' efficacy and control beliefs regarding memory and aging: Separating general from personal beliefs. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 5(4), 264–296. doi: 10.1076/anec.5.4.264.771
  • Maggio, C., Soubelet, A., Faure, S., & Fort, I. (2019). The relationships between perceived control and episodic memory in adulthood: A review. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 26(2), 222–243. doi:10.1080/13825585.2017.1423022
  • Mello, Z. R., & Worrell, F. C. (2007). The adolescent and adult time inventory - English. Unpublished scale. Berkeley, CA: The University of California.
  • Mello, Z. R., & Worrell, F. C. (2010). The adolescent and adult time inventory: Preliminary technical manual. Colorado Springs, CO & Berkeley, CA. Retrieved from https://faculty.sfsu.edu/sites/default/files/faculty_files/4810/ATI_V3.1.Manual.pdf
  • Mello, Z. R., Zhang, J. W., Barber, S. J., Paoloni, V. C., Howell, R. T., & Worrell, F. C. (2016). Psychometric properties of time attitude scores in young, middle, and older adult samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 57–61. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.037
  • Miller, L. M. S., & Lachman, M. E. (1999). The sense of control and cognitive aging: Toward a model of mediational processes. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (pp. 17–41). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Neugarten, B. (1968). Middle age and aging: A reader in social psychology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Nurmi, J. E. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11(1), 1–59. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(91)90002-6
  • O’Brien, R. M. (2007). A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity, 41(5), 673–690. doi:10.1007/s11135-006-9018-6
  • Parisi, J. M., Gross, A. L., Marsiske, M., Willis, S. L., & Rebok, G. W. (2017). Control beliefs and cognition over a 10-year period: Findings from the ACTIVE trial. Psychology and Aging, 32(1), 69–75. doi:10.1037/pag0000147
  • Park, I., Rie, J., Kim, H. S., & Park, J. (2018). Effects of a future time perspective-based career intervention on career decisions. Journal of Career Development. doi:10.1177/0894845318781043
  • Payne, B. R., Gross, A. L., Hill, P. L., Parisi, J. M., Rebok, G. W., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2017). Decomposing the relationship between cognitive functioning and self-referent memory beliefs in older adulthood: What’s memory got to do with it? Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24(4), 345–362. doi:10.1080/13825585.2016.1218425
  • Pulkkinen, L., & Rönkä, A. (1994). Personal control over development, identity formation, and future orientation as components of life orientation: A developmental approach. Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 260–271. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.260
  • Rodin, J., Rennert, K., & Solomon, S. K. (1980). Intrinsic motivation for control. Fact or fiction? In A. Baum & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology (pp. 131–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Rohr, M. K., John, D. T., Fung, H. H., & Lang, F. R. (2017). A three-component model of future time perspective across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 32(7), 597–607. doi:10.1037/pag0000191
  • Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A lifespan model of successful aging. American Psychologist, 51(7), 702–714.
  • Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, & coping. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 549–570.
  • Soederberg, L. M., & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Cognitive performance and the role of control beliefs in midlife. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7(2), 69–85. doi:10.1076/1382-5585(200006)7:2;1-U;FT069
  • Strickland-Hughes, C. M., West, R. L., Smith, K. A., & Ebner, N. C. (2016). False feedback and beliefs influence name recall in younger and older adults. Memory, 25(8), 1072–1088. doi:10.1080/09658211.2016.1260746
  • Tasdemir-Ozdes, A., Strickland-Hughes, C. M., Bluck, S., & Ebner, N. C. (2016). Future perspective and healthy lifestyle choices in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 618–630. doi:10.1037/pag0000089
  • Trommsdorff, G. (1994). Future time perspective and control orientation: Social conditions and consequences. In Z. Zaleski (Ed.), Psychology of future orientation (pp. 39−62). Lublin, Poland: Towarzystwo Naukowe KUL.
  • Weiss, D., Job, V., Mathias, M., Grah, S., & Freund, A. M. (2016). The end is (not) near: Aging, essentialism, and future time perspective. Developmental Psychology, 52(6), 996–1009. doi:10.1037/dev0000115
  • West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K., & Dark-Freudeman, A. (2008). Self-efficacy and memory aging: The impact of a memory intervention based on self-efficacy. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15(3), 302–329. doi:10.1080/13825580701440510
  • West, R. L., & Yassuda, M. S. (2004). Aging and memory control beliefs: Performance in relation to goal setting and memory self-evaluation. Journals of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 59(2), 56–65. doi:10.1093/geronb/59.2.P56
  • Windsor, T. D., & Anstey, K. J. (2008). A longitudinal investigation of perceived control and cognitive performance in young, midlife and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15(6), 744–763. doi:10.1080/13825580802348570
  • Wolf, E. J., Harrington, K. M., Clark, S. L., & Miller, M. W. (2015). Sample size requirements for structural equation models: An evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 76(6), 913–934.
  • Wolinsky, F. D., Vander Weg, M. W., Martin, R., Unverzagt, F. W., Willis, S. L., Marsiske, M., ... Tennstedt, S. L. (2009). Does cognitive training improve internal locus of control among older adults? Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65(5), 591–598. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp117
  • Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2009). Remaining time and opportunities at work: Relationships between age, work characteristics, and occupational future time perspective. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 487–493. doi:10.1037/a0015425

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.