1,397
Views
22
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Maternal Expansions of Child Language Relate to Growth in Children’s Vocabulary

References

  • Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Useful evidence on negative evidence. In R. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Becoming a word learning: A debate on lexical acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Bohannon, J., Padgett, R., Nelson, K. E., & Mark, M. (1996). Useful evidence on negative evidence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 551–555. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.551
  • Bornstein, M., Haynes, M., & Painter, K. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–393. doi:10.1017/S0305000998003456
  • Bornstein, M., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Hahn, C., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44 (3), 867–874. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.867
  • Bryk, A. S., Raudenbush, S. W., & Congdon, R. T. (1996–2011). Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling with HLM/2L and HLM/3L programs. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
  • Chouinard, M. M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30 (03), 637–669. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005701
  • Cross, T. (1978). Mothers’ speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In N. Waterson, & C. E. Snow (Eds.), The development of communication. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Demetras, M., Nolan Post, K., & Snow, C. E. (1986). Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language, 13, 275–292. doi:10.1017/S0305000900008059
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
  • Edwards, S., Fletcher, P., Garman, M., Hughes, A., Letts, C., & Sinka, I. (1997). The Reynell developmental scales III: The university of reading edition. London, UK: NFER-Nelson.
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznik, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartnung, J., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The McArthur communicative development inventories. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Bates, E., & Reznick, J. S. (2007). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Fey, M., Krulik, T., Loeb, D., & Proctor-Williams, K. (1999). Sentence recast use by parents of children with typical language and children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology, 8 (3), 273–286. doi:10.1044/1058-0360.0803.273
  • Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J. (1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73 (2), 135–176. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00036-0
  • Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitzman, E., Zocconi, E., & Pearce, P. (2002). Mother-child interactions in Canada and Italy: Linguistic responsiveness to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37, 153–171. doi:10.1080/13682820110116794
  • Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Patsy Steig, P. (1999). The relationship between maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology, 8 (4), 364. doi:10.1044/1058-0360.0804.364
  • Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. doi:10.1207/s15327817la0101_2
  • Goldstein, M., Schwade, J., & Bornstein, M. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80, 636–644. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01287.x
  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: P.H. Brookes.
  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Treiman, R., & Schneiderman, M. (1984). Brown & Hanlon revisited: Mothers’ sensitivity to ungrammatical forms. Journal of Child Language, 11, 81–88. doi:10.1017/S0305000900005596
  • Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73 (2), 418–433. doi:10.1111/cdev.2002.73.issue-2
  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child’s development of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 22 (2), 155–163. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.2.155
  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1990). Maternal speech and the child’s development of syntax: A further look. Journal of Child Language, 17 (1), 85–99. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013118
  • Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27 (2), 236–248. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236
  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00500-5
  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43 (5), 1062–1083. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1062
  • Livickis, P., Reilly, S., Girolametto, L., Ukoumunne, O., & Wake, M. (2014). Maternal behaviors promoting language acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers: Prospective community-based study. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 274–281. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000056
  • Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (1998). SALT: Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (Version 6.0). Madison, WI: Language analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin.
  • Morgan, J., Bonamo, K., & Travis, L. (1995). Negative evidence on negative evidence. Developmental Psychology, 31, 180–197. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.2.180
  • Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17 (02), 357–374. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013817
  • Naigles, L., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1995). Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 31 (5), 827–837. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.827
  • Naigles, L., & Svenson, L. (2009). Syntactic supports for word learning. In E. Hoff, & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development. Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Naigles, L. R. (1996). The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping. Cognition, 58 (2), 221–251. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00681-8
  • Nelson, K. E., & Bonvillian, J. (1973). Concepts and words in the 18-month-old: Acquiring concept names under controlled conditions. Cognition, 2 (4), 435–450. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(73)90003-6
  • Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York, NY: CUP.
  • Ochs, E. (1982). Talking to children in Western Samoa. Language in Society, 11, 77–104. doi:10.1017/S0047404500009040
  • Onnis, L., Waterfall, H., & Edelman, S. (2011). Lean locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues. Cognition, 109, 423–430. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.004
  • Pan, B. A., Rowe, M., Spier, E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2004). Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low-income families: Concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of data. Journal of Child Language, 31, 587–608. doi:10.1017/S0305000904006270
  • Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76 (4), 763–782.
  • Proctor-Williams, K., & Fey, M. (2007). Recast density and acquisiton of novel irregular past tense verbs. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 50, 1029–1047. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/072)
  • Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2014). Skype Me! socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85 (3), 956–970. doi:10.1111/cdev.12166
  • Rowe, M. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008343
  • Rowe, M. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83, 1762–1774. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x
  • Saxton, M., Kulcsaar, B., Marshall, G., & Rupra, M. (1998). Longer-term effects of corrective input: An experimental approach. Journal of Child Language, 25, 701–721. doi:10.1017/S0305000998003559
  • Shimpi, P., Fedewa, A., & Sydney, H. (2012). Social and linguistic input in low-income African American mother–child dyads from 1 month through 2 years: Relations to vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 (04), 781–798. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000567
  • Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science, 15 (5), 659–673. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01168.x
  • Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.). (1977). Talking to children. Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univeristy Press.
  • Tamis-LeMonda, C., Kuchirko, Y., & Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23 (2), 121–126. doi:10.1177/0963721414522813
  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72 (3), 748–767. doi:10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-3
  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465–481.
  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57 (6), 1454–1463. doi:10.2307/1130423
  • Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4 (12), 197–211. doi:10.1177/014272378300401202
  • Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24 (11), 2143–2152. doi:10.1177/0956797613488145
  • Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical output as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37 (2), 265–279. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.265
  • Yoder, P. J. (2006). Predicting lexical density growth rate in young children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15 (4), 378–388. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2006/035)

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.