566
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Feeling the Way to Words: Parents’ Speech and Touch Cues Highlight Word-To-World Mappings of Body Parts

, , , &

References

  • Abu-Zhaya, R., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2017). Multimodal infant-directed communication: How caregivers combine tactile and linguistic cues. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1088–1116. doi:10.1017/S0305000916000416
  • Altvater-Mackensen, N., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Learning to match auditory and visual speech cues: Social influences on acquisition of phonological categories. Child Development, 86(2), 362–378. doi:10.1111/cdev.12320
  • Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2000). Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 190–201. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.190
  • Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30, 153–187. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(02)80041-6
  • Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253–3258. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113380109
  • Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2015). Early word comprehension in infants: Replication and extension. Language Learning and Development, 11(4), 369–380. doi:10.1080/15475441.2014.979387
  • Best, C. T., Goldstein, L. M., Nam, H., & Tyler, M. D. (2016). Articulating what infants attune to in native speech. Ecological Psychology, 28(4), 216–261. doi:10.1080/10407413.2016.1230372
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2014). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5. 3.84). Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/
  • Brand, R. J., & Tapscott, S. (2007). Acoustic packaging of action sequences by infants. Infancy, 11(3), 321–332. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00230.x
  • Brugman, H., & Russel, A. (2004). Annotating multimedia/multi-modal resources with ELAN. In M. T. Lino, M. F. Xavier, F. Ferreira, R. Costa, & R. Silva (Eds.), Proceedings of LREC 2004. Paris, France: ELRA - European Language Resources Association. Retrieved from http://www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2004/
  • Bruner, J. S., & Watson, R. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
  • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149–219. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1
  • Carra, C., Lavelli, M., & Keller, H. (2014). Differences in practices of body stimulation during the first 3 months: Ethnotheories and behaviors of Italian mothers and West African immigrant mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(1), 5–15. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.10.004
  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37–46. doi:10.1177/001316446002000104
  • Feldman, R., & Reznick, J. S. (1996). Maternal perception of infant intentionality at 4 and 8 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(4), 483–496. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90008-9
  • Fenson, L. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., … Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5). doi:10.2307/1166093
  • Ferber, S. G., Feldman, R., & Makhoul, I. R. (2008). The development of maternal touch across the first year of life. Early Human Development, 84(6), 363–370. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.09.019
  • Field, T. (2001). Touch. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
  • Franco, F., Fogel, A., Messinger, D. S., & Frazier, C. A. (1996). Cultural differences in physical contact between hispanic and anglo mother–Infant dyads living in the United States. Early Development and Parenting, 5(3), 119–127. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199609)5:3<119::AID-EDP123>3.0.CO;2-Y
  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2–18. doi:10.1037/a0024338
  • Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39(1), 1–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.000245
  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Gogate, L., & Hollich, G. (2016). Early verb-action and noun-object mapping across sensory modalities: A neuro-developmental view. Developmental Neuropsychology, 41(5–8), 293–307. doi:10.1080/87565641.2016.1243112
  • Gogate, L., Maganti, M., & Bahrick, L. E. (2015). Cross-cultural evidence for multimodal motherese: Asian Indian mothers’ adaptive use of synchronous words and gestures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 110–126. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.002
  • Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., & Watson, J. D. (2000). A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development, 71(4), 878–894. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00197
  • Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. H., & Betancourt, E. A. (2006). Attention to maternal multimodal naming by 6- to 8-month-old infants and learning of word–Object relations. Infancy, 9(3), 259–288. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0903_1
  • Gogate, L. J., & Hollich, G. (2010). Invariance detection within an interactive system: A perceptual gateway to language development. Psychological Review, 117(2), 496–516. doi:10.1037/a0019049
  • Gogate, L. J., Maganti, M., & Laing, K. B. (2013). Maternal naming of object wholes versus parts to preverbal infants: A fine-grained analysis of scaffolding at 6–8 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(3), 470–479. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.03.012
  • Gogate, L. J., Walker-Andrews, A. S., & Bahrick, L. E. (2001). The intersensory origins of word-comprehension: An ecological-dynamic systems view. Developmental Science, 4(1), 1–18. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00143
  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Baby wordsmith: From associationist to social sophisticate. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(1), 30–33. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00401.x
  • Gordon, I., Voos, A. C., Bennett, R. H., Bolling, D. Z., Pelphrey, K. A., & Kaiser, M. D. (2013). Brain mechanisms for processing affective touch. Human Brain Mapping, 34(4), 914–922. doi:10.1002/hbm.21480
  • Hepper, P. G. (2002). Prenatal development. In A. Slater & M. Lewis (Eds.), Introduction to infant development (pp. 39–60). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Hertenstein, M. J. (2002). Touch: Its communicative functions in infancy. Human Development, 45(2), 70–94. doi:10.1159/000048154
  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
  • Hooker, D. (1952). The prenatal origin of behavior. Lawrence, Kansas: University of Kansas Press. Retrieved from http://kuscholarworks.ku.edu/handle/1808/6341
  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37(2), 229. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990432
  • Jean, A. D. L., Stack, D. M., & Fogel, A. (2009). A longitudinal investigation of maternal touching across the first 6 months of life: Age and context effects. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(3), 344–349. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.04.005
  • Jesse, A., & Johnson, E. K. (2012). Prosodic temporal alignment of co-speech gestures to speech facilitates referent resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(6), 1567–1581. doi:10.1037/a0027921
  • Johnson, E. K., Seidl, A., & Tyler, M. D. (2014). The edge factor in early word segmentation: Utterance-level prosody enables word form extraction by 6-month-olds. PLoS One, 9(1), e83546. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083546
  • Koenig, M., & Cole, C. (2013). Early word learning. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 492–503). New York, NY: OUP.
  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. doi:10.2307/2529310
  • Leclère, C., Viaux, S., Avril, M., Achard, C., Chetouani, M., Missonnier, S., & Cohen, D. (2014). Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: A systematic review. PLoS One, 9(12), e113571. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0113571
  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). The development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 281. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.281
  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2002). Heterogeneity and heterochrony in the development of intersensory perception. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(1), 41–63. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00060-5
  • Mandler, J. M. (2015). Attention as the origin of meaning formation. In G. Marchetti (Ed.), Attention and meaning: The attentional basis of meaning (pp. 273–289). Hauppauge, NY, US: Nova Science Publishers. Retrieved from eBook Collection (EBSCOhost).
  • Maouene, J., Hidaka, S., & Smith, L. (2008). Body parts and early-learned verbs. Cognitive Science, 32(7), 1200–1216. doi:10.1080/03640210802019997
  • Matatyaho, D. J., & Gogate, L. J. (2008). Type of maternal object motion during synchronous naming predicts preverbal infants’ learning of word–Object relations. Infancy, 13(2), 172–184. doi:10.1080/15250000701795655
  • Matatyaho-Bullaro, D. J., Gogate, L., Mason, Z., Cadavid, S., & Abdel-Mottaleb, M. (2014). Type of object motion facilitates word mapping by preverbal infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118, 27–40. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.010
  • Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. (2013). ELAN (Version 4.6.0). Nijmegen, The Netherlands: The Language Archive. Retrieved from http://tla.mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/
  • McHugh, M. L. (2012). Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia Medica, 22(3), 276–282. doi:10.11613/BM.2012.031
  • Meyer, M., Hard, B., Brand, R. J., McGarvey, M., & Baldwin, D. A. (2011). Acoustic packaging: Maternal speech and action synchrony. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3(2), 154–162. doi:10.1109/TAMD.2010.2103941
  • Morrison, I., Löken, L. S., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 305–314. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2007-y
  • Nomikou, I., Koke, M., & Rohlfing, K. J. (2017). Verbs in mothers’ input to six-month-olds: Synchrony between presentation, meaning, and actions is related to later verb acquisition. Brain Sciences, 7(5), 52. doi:10.3390/brainsci7050052
  • Nomikou, I., & Rohlfing, K. J. (2011). Language does something: Body action and language in maternal input to three-month-olds. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3(2), 113–128. doi:10.1109/TAMD.2011.2140113
  • Pelaez-Nogueras, M., Gewirtz, J. L., Field, T., Cigales, M., Malphurs, J., Clasky, S., & Sanchez, A. (1996). Infants’ preference for touch stimulation in face-to-face interactions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 199–213. doi:10.1016/S0193-3973(96)90025-8
  • Quinn, P. C., & Eimas, P. D. (1996). Perceptual organization and categorization in young infants. In C. Rovee-Collier & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 10, pp. 1–36). Westport, CT, US: Ablex Publishing.
  • R Development Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/
  • Rader, N. de V., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2012). Caregivers’ gestures direct infant attention during early word learning: The importance of dynamic synchrony. Language Sciences, 34(5), 559–568. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.011
  • Rader, N. de V., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2010). How the hands control attention during early word learning. Gesture, 10(2–3), 202–221. doi:10.1075/gest.10.2-3.05rad
  • Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2000). Perceived self in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(3–4), 513–530. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00055-8
  • Seidl, A., Tincoff, R., Baker, C., & Cristia, A. (2015). Why the body comes first: Effects of experimenter touch on infants’ word finding. Developmental Science, 18(1), 155–164. doi:10.1111/desc.12182
  • Smith, L. B. (2005). Cognition as a dynamic system: Principles from embodiment. Developmental Review, 25(3–4), 278–298. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.001
  • Smith, L. B., Maouene, J., & Hidaka, S. (2007). The body and children’s word learning. In J. Plumert & J. Spencer (Eds.), The emerging spatial mind (pp. 168–192). New York, NY: OUP.
  • Smith, L. B., Suanda, S. H., & Yu, C. (2014). The unrealized promise of infant statistical word–Referent learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 251–258. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.007
  • Snow, C., de Blauw, A., & Van Roosmalen, G. (1979). Talking and playing with babies: The role of ideologies of child-rearing. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 269–288). Cambridge, England: CUP.
  • Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.). (1977). Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Stack, D. M., & Jean, A. D. L. (2011). Communicating through touch: Touching during parent-infant interactions. In M. J. Hertenstein & S. J. Weiss (Eds.),The handbook of touch: Neuroscience, behavioral, and health perspectives (pp. 273–298). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company, LLC.
  • Stack, D. M., & Muir, D. W. (1992). Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development, 63(6), 1509. doi:10.2307/1131572
  • Sullivan, J. W., & Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal perception and maternal multimodal stimulation: Implications for language development. Advances in Infancy Research, 2, 183–239.
  • Swingley, D. (2009). Contributions of infant word learning to language development. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3617–3632. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0107
  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Song, L. (2012). Parent-infant communicative interactions in cultural context. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, J. Mistry, & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 6: Developmental psychology (2nd ed., pp. 143–170). Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  • Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10(2), 172–175. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00127
  • Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2012). Six-month-olds comprehend words that refer to parts of the body. Infancy, 17(4), 432–444. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x
  • Vallbo, A., Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., & Norrsell, U. (1993). A system of unmyelinated afferents for innocuous mechanoreception in the human skin. Brain Research, 628(1–2), 301–304. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(93)90968-S
  • van de Weijer, J. (1998). Language input for word discovery. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Max Planck Institute.
  • Waxman, S. R., & Lidz, J. L. (2006). Early word learning. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 299–335). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125(2), 244–262. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.016
  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Joint attention without gaze following: Human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. PLoS One, 8(11), e79659. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079659
  • Zukow, P. G. (1989). Siblings as effective socializing agents: Evidence from central Mexico. In P. G. Zukow (Ed.), Sibling interaction across cultures: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 79–104). New York, NY: Springer Verlag.
  • Zukow, P. G. (1990). Socio-perceptual bases for the emergence of language: An alternative to innatist approaches. Developmental Psychobiology, 23(7), 705–726. doi:10.1002/dev.420230711
  • Zukow-Goldring, P. (1997). A social ecological realist approach to the emergence of the lexicon: Educating attention to amodal invariants to gesture and speech. In C. Dent-Read & P. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: Ecological approaches to the organism-environment systems (pp. 199–250). Washington, D.C.: APA.
  • Zukow-Goldring, P., & Rader, N. de V., (2001). Perceiving referring actions: A commentary on Gogate, Walker-Andrews, and Bahrick. Developmental Science, 4(1), 28–30. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00144

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.