353
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Negative emotional stimuli reduce contextual cueing but not response times in inefficient search

, , &
Pages 377-393 | Received 29 Aug 2012, Published online: 12 Jul 2013

REFERENCES

  • Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2003). Motion onset captures attention. Psychological Science, 14, 427–432. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01458
  • Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2005). The onset of receding motion captures attention: Comment on Franconeri & Simons (2003). Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 219–223. doi: 10.3758/BF03206486
  • Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.
  • Becker, M. W. (2009). Panic search: Fear produces efficient visual search for nonthreatening objects. Psychological Science, 20, 435–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02303.x
  • Blagrove, E., & Watson, D. G. (2010). Visual marking and facial affect: Can an emotional face be ignored? Emotion, 10, 147–168. doi: 10.1037/a0017743
  • Brady, T. F., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Spatial constraints on learning in visual search: Modeling contextual cueing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33(4), 798–815.
  • Buchanan, T. W., & Adolphs, R. (2002). The role of the human amygdala in emotional modulation of long-term declarative memory. In S. Moore & M. Oaksford (Eds.), Emotional cognition: From brain to behavior (pp. 9–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.
  • Burton, L. A., Rabin, L., Vardy, S. B., Frohlich, J., Wyatt, G., Dimitri, D., … Guterman, E. (2004). Gender differences in implicit and explicit memory for affective passages. Brain and Cognition, 54, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.011
  • Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theories of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 231–248. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231
  • Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 28–71. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
  • Collins, M. A., & Cooke, A. (2005). A transfer investigating implicit memory for emotional words. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1529–1545. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.029
  • Dewhurst, S. A., & Parry, L. A. (2000). Emotionality, distinctiveness, and recollective experience. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 541–551. doi: 10.1080/095414400750050222
  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433
  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organisation of behaviour. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201. doi: 10.1037/h0047707
  • Fenske, M. J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: Evidence from flanker tasks. Emotion, 3, 327–343. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.327
  • Flykt, A. (2006). Preparedness for action: Responding to the snake in the grass. The American Journal of Psychology, 119, 29–43. doi: 10.2307/20445317
  • Fox, E., Griggs, L., & Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: Are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion, 7, 691–696. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359, 1367–1377. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
  • Frischen, A., Eastwood, J. D., & Smilek, D. (2008). Visual search for faces with emotional expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 662–676. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.5.662
  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359622
  • Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01707-1
  • Huguet, P., Galvaing, M. P., Monteil, J. M., & Dumas, F. (1999). Social presence effects in the Stroop task: Further evidence for an attentional view of social facilitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1011
  • Jiang, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2001). Selective attention modulates implicit learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(4), 1105–1124.
  • Johnson, J. S., Woodman, G. F., Braun, E., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 834–839. doi: 10.3758/BF03194108
  • Kapp, B. S., Whalen, P. J., Supple, W. F., & Pascoe, J. P. (1992). Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned arousal information processing. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss.
  • Klein, R. M. (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature, 334, 430–431. doi: 10.1038/334430a0
  • Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S. J., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Does contextual cueing guide the deployment of attention? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 816–828.
  • Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S. J., & Wolfe, J. M. (2006). Contextual cueing by global features. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1204–1216. doi: 10.3758/BF03193721
  • Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S. J., & Wolfe, J. M. (2008a). The role of memory and restricted context in repeated visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 314–328. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.2.314
  • Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S. J., & Wolfe, J. M. (2008b). Time to guide: Evidence for delayed attentional guidance in contextual cueing. Visual Cognition, 16, 804–825. doi: 10.1080/13506280701751224
  • Kunar, M. A., Humphreys, G. W., Smith, K. J., & Hulleman, J. (2003). What is marked in visual marking?: Evidence for effects of configuration in preview search. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 982–996. doi: 10.3758/BF03194828
  • Kunar, M. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (2011). Target absent trials in configural contextual cueing. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2077–2091. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0164-0
  • LaBar, K. S., Torpey, D. C., Cook, C. A., Johnson, S. R., Warren, L. H., Burke, J., & Bohmer, K. A. (2005). Emotional enhancement of perceptual priming is preserved in aging and early-stage Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1824–1837. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.018
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2001). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-5). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.
  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces–KDEF [CD-ROM]. Karolinska Institute, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology Section.
  • McLeod, P., Driver, J., & Crisp, J. (1988). Visual-search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel. Nature, 332, 154–155. doi: 10.1038/332154a0
  • Michael, T., Ehlers, A., & Halligan, S. L. (2005). Enhanced priming for trauma-related material in posttraumatic stress disorder. Emotion, 5, 103–112. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.103
  • Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 654–661. doi: 10.3758/BF03196754
  • Most, S. B., & Wang, L. (2011). Dissociating spatial attention and awareness in emotion-induced blindness. Psychological Science, 22, 300–305. doi: 10.1177/0956797610397665
  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
  • Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are affective events richly “remembered” or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 242–261. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.242
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Olatunji, B. O., Ciesielski, B., Armstrong, T., & Zald, D. (2011). Emotional expressions and visual search efficiency: Specificity and effects of anxiety symptoms. Emotion, 11(5), 1073–1079. doi: 10.1037/a0021785
  • Olivers, C. N. L., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2006). The beneficial effects of additional task load, positive affect, and instruction on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 364–379.
  • Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2002). Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context. Visual Cognition, 9(3), 273–302. doi: 10.1080/13506280042000162
  • Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety: Different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298–304. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359624
  • Peterson, M. S., & Kramer, A. F. (2001). Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1239–1249. doi: 10.3758/BF03194537
  • Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48, 175–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025
  • Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefit of attention. Psychological Science, 17, 292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x
  • Sanders, G. S., Baron, R. S., & Moore, D. (1978). Distraction and social comparison as mediators of social facilitation effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 291–303. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(78)90017-3
  • Schankin, A., & Schubö, A. (2009). Cognitive processes facilitated by contextual cueing. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 46, 668–679. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00807.x
  • Smilek, D., Enns, J., Eastwood, J., & Merikle, P. (2006). Relax! Cognitive strategy influences visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 543–564. doi: 10.1080/13506280500193487
  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). Feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2
  • Wachtel, P. L. (1968). Anxiety, attention, and coping with threat. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 137–143. doi: 10.1037/h0020118
  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 89–101. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9021-1
  • Watson, D. G., & Humphreys, G. W. (1997). Visual marking: Prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. Psychological Review, 104(1), 90–122. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.90
  • Watson, D. G., & Humphreys, G. W. (1998). Visual marking of moving objects: A role for top-down feature based inhibition in selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 946–962.
  • Watson, D. G., Maylor, E. A., & Bruce, L. A. M. (2005). Search, enumeration and aging: Eye movement requirements cause age-equivalent performance in enumeration but not in search tasks. Psychology and Aging, 20, 226–240. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.226
  • Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Guided Search 4.0: Current progress with a model of visual search. In W. Gray (Ed.), Integrated models of cognitive systems (pp. 99–119). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 419–433.
  • Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/nrn1411
  • Wolfe, J. M., Klempen, N., & Dahlen, K. (2000). Postattentive vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 693–716.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.