325
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Time course analyses of orthographic and phonological priming effects during word recognition in a transparent orthography

, , &
Pages 1925-1943 | Received 28 Jun 2012, Accepted 20 Nov 2013, Published online: 03 Mar 2014

REFERENCES

  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. (1993). CELEX lexical database [Computer software]. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
  • Bick, A. S., Goelman, G., & Frost, R. (2011). Hebrew brain vs. English brain: language modulates the way it is processed. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2280–2290. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21583
  • Braun, M., Hutzler, F., Ziegler, J. C., Dambacher, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Pseudohomophone effects provide evidence of early lexico-phonological processing in visual word recognition. Human Brain Mapping, 30(7), 1977–1989. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20643
  • Brus, B. T., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1979). Een-minuut-test, vorm A en B. Verantwoording en Handleiding [One-minute-test, form A and B]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Brysbaert, M. (2001). Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: Automatic after all. Memory & Cognition, 29(5), 765–773. doi: 10.3758/BF03200479
  • Brysbaert, M. (2003). Bilingual visual word recognition: Evidence from masked phonological priming. In S. Kinoshita & S. J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 323–343). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Brysbaert, M., & Praet, C. (1992). Reading isolated words : No evidence for automatic incorporation of the phonetic code. Psychological Research, 54(2), 91–102. doi: 10.1007/BF00937137
  • Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & Perea, M. (2005). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Research Report, 16(8), 585–589.
  • Carreiras, M., Perea, M., Vergara, M., & Pollatsek, A. (2009). The time course of orthography and phonology: ERP correlates of masked priming effects in Spanish. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 1113–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00844.x
  • Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. I. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 165–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.006
  • Comesaña, M., Sánchez-Casas, R., Soares, A. P., Pinheiro, A. P., Rauber, A., Frade, S., & Fraga, I. (2012). The interplay of phonology and orthography in visual cognate word recognition: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 529(1), 75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.010
  • Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2011a). Transliteration and transcription effects in bi-scriptal readers: The case of Greeklish. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 729–735. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0095-8
  • Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2011b). Phonology by itself: Masked phonological priming effects with and without orthographic overlap. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(2), 185–203. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.477811
  • Durgunoğlu, A. Y., & Öney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing, 11(4), 281–299. doi: 10.1023/A:1008093232622
  • Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 488–499. doi: 10.3758/BF03195595
  • Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence from masked non-word priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 45(3), 353–372. doi: 10.1080/02724989208250619
  • Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1993). The time course of orthographic and phonological code activation in the early phases of visual word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(2), 119–122. doi: 10.3758/BF03334157
  • Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1994). Effects of orthography are independent of phonology in masked form priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(2), 365–382. doi: 10.1080/14640749408401116
  • Forster, K. I. & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10(4), 680–698.
  • Forster, K. I., Mohan, K., & Hector, J. (2003). The mechanics of masked priming. In S. Kinoshita & S. J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 3–37). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 71–99. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.71
  • Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic systems and skilled word recognition processes in reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 272–295). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Frost, R., Ahissar, M., Gotesman, R., & Tayeb, S. (2003). Are phonological effects fragile? The effect of luminance and exposure duration on form priming and phonological priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 346–378. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00510-7
  • Frost, R., Forster, K. I., & Deutsch, A. (1997). What can we learn from the morphology of Hebrew? A masked priming investigation of morphological representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23(4), 829–856.
  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & Fraca de Barrera, L. (1998). Children's orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 19–52. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010560
  • Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2009). Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 128–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00121.x
  • Grainger, J., Kiyonaga, K., & Holcomb, P. J. (2006). The time course of orthographic and phonological code activation. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1021–1026. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01821.x
  • Gronau, N., & Frost, R. (1997). Pre-lexical phonological computation in a deep orthography: Evidence from backward masking in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1), 107–112. doi: 10.3758/BF03210781
  • Johnston, M., & Castles, A. (2003). Dissociating automatic orthographic and phonological codes in lexical access and lexical acquisition. In S. Kinoshita & S. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 193–222). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Katz, L., & Feldman, L. B. (1983). Relation between pronunciation and recognition of printed words in deep and shallow orthographies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 157–166.
  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning: an overview. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 1–8). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
  • Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: A new measure for Dutch word frequency based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 643–650. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.643
  • Kinoshita, S., & Lupker, S. J. (Eds.). (2003). Masked priming: The state of the art. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Nederlandse Taalunie [Dutch Language Asssociation]. (2005). Het groene boekje. Woordenlijst der Nederlandse taal [Glossary of the Dutch Langage]. Den Haag, The Netherlands: SDU Uitgevers/Lannoo.
  • Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 829–841. doi: 10.3758/BF03196438
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 101–118.
  • Perry, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2002). On the nature of phonological assembly: Evidence from backward masking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(1), 31–59. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000157
  • Pollatsek, A., & Well, A. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(3), 785–794.
  • Protopapas, A., & Vlahou, E. L. (2009). A comparative quantitative analysis of Greek orthographic transparency. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 991–1008. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.991
  • Rastle, K. (2007). Visual word recognition. In M. G. Gaskell (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 71–87). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Masked phonological priming effects in English: Are they real? Do they matter? Cognitive Psychology, 53(2), 97–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.01.002
  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British journal of psychology, 94(2), 143–74. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859
  • Van Den Bos, K. P., Lutje Spelberg, H. C., Scheepstra, A. J. M., & De Vries, J. R. (1994). De Klepel: Een test voor de leesvaardigheid van pseudowoorden [The Klepel: A test of reading skills for pseudowords]. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Van Orden, G. C., & Kloos, H. (2005). The question of phonology and reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 61–78). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
  • Ziegler, J. C., Grainger, J., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Modelling word recognition and reading aloud. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(5), 641–649. doi: 10.1080/09541446.2010.496263
  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12(3), 413–430. doi: 10.1080/09541440050114570
  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Braun, M. (2001). Identical words are read differently in different languages. Psychological science, 12(5), 379–384. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00370

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.