1,025
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Encoding order and developmental dyslexia: A family of skills predicting different orthographic components

, &
Pages 99-128 | Received 11 Mar 2013, Accepted 06 Jun 2014, Published online: 23 Sep 2014

REFERENCES

  • Au, A., & Lovegrove, B. (2007). The contribution of rapid visual and auditory processing to the reading of irregular words and pseudowords presented singly and in contiguity. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1344–1359. doi: 10.3758/BF03192951
  • Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, L. (1994). Doors and people: A test of visual and verbal recall and recognition. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Ben-Artzi, E., Fostick, L., & Babkoff, H. (2005). Deficits in temporal-order judgements in dyslexia: Evidence from diotic stimuli differing spectrally and from dichotic stimuli differing only by perceived location. Neuropsychologia, 43, 714–723. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.004
  • Ben-Yehudah, G., & Ahissar, M. (2004). Sequential spatial frequency discrimination is consistently impaired among adult dyslexics. Vision Research, 44, 1047–1063. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.12.001
  • Ben-Yehudah, G., Sackett, E., Malchi-Ginzberg, L., & Ahissar, M. (2001). Impaired temporal contrast sensitivity in dyslexics is specific to retain-and-compare paradigms. Brain, 124, 1381–1395. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.7.1381
  • Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 36, 2257–2273. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.007
  • Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M.-J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009
  • Breznitz, Z., & Meyler, A. (2003). Speed of lower-level auditory and visual processing as a basic factor in dyslexia: Electrophysiological evidence. Brain and Language, 85, 166–184. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00513-8
  • Brownstein, S., Weiner, M., & Weiner-Green, S. (1997). How to prepare for SAT1. Hauppauge, NY: Barron's Educational Series, Icnc.
  • Buchholz, J., & Aimola-Davies, A. (2007). Attentional blink deficits observed in dyslexia depend on task demands. Vision Research, 47, 1292–1302. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.11.028
  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read?. Cognition, 91, 77–111. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00164-1
  • Cestnick, L., & Coltheart, M. (1999). The relationship between language-processing and visual-processing deficits in developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 71(3), 231–255. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00023-2
  • Conlon, E., Sanders, M., & Zapart, S. (2004). Temporal processing in poor adult readers. Neuropsychologia, 42, 142–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.07.004
  • Davis, C., Castles, A., McAnally, K., & Gray, J. (2001). Lapses of concentration and dyslexic performance on the Ternus task. Cognition, 81(2), B21–B31. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00129-9
  • Di Betta, A.-M., & Romani, C. (2006). Lexical learning and dysgraphia in a group of adults with developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(3), 376–400. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000545
  • Eden, G., Stein, J., Wood, H., & Wood, F. (1995). Temporal and spatial processing in reading disabled and normal children. Cortex, 31, 451–468. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80059-7
  • Facoetti, A., Ruffino, M., Peru, A., Paganoni, P., & Chelazzi, L. (2008). Sluggish engagement and disengagement of non-spatial attention in dyslexic children. Cortex, 44, 1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.007
  • Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., … Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1011–1025. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232
  • Facoetti, A., Zorzi, M., Cestnick, L., Lorusso, M.-L., Molteni, M., Paganoni, P., … Mascetti, G. G. (2006). The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 841–855. doi: 10.1080/02643290500483090
  • Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, p460–493. doi: 10.3758/BF03210983
  • Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 814–819. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013
  • Glasspool, D. W., & Houghton, G. (2005). Serial order and consonant-vowel structure in a graphemic output buffer model. Brain and Language, 94, 304–330. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.01.006
  • Glasspool, D. W., Shallice, T., & Cipolotti, L. (2006). Towards a unified process model for graphemic buffer disorder and deep dysgraphia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 479–512. doi: 10.1080/02643290500265109
  • Hansen, P. C., Stein, J. F., Orde, S. R., Winter, J. L., & Talcott, J. B. (2001). Are dyslexics’ visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function?. Neuroreport, 12(7), 1527–1530. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105250-00045
  • Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525–532. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01801-5
  • Hatcher, J., Snowling, M. J., & Griffiths, J. M. (2002). Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 119–133. doi: 10.1348/000709902158801
  • Hawelka, S., Huber, C., & Wimmer, H. (2006). Impaired visual processing of letter and digit strings in adult dyslexic readers. Vision Research, 46, 718–723. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.09.017
  • Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45, 855–863. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.007
  • Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Visual target detection is not impaired in dyslexic readers. Vision Research, 48, 850–852. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.003
  • Herrman, J. A., Matyas, T., & Pratt, C. (2006). Meta-analysis of the nonword reading deficit in Specific Rading Disorder. Dyslexia, 12, 195–221. doi: 10.1002/dys.324
  • Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65–70.
  • Hill, G. T., & Raymond, J. E. (2002). Deficits of motion transparency perception in adult developmental dyslexics with normal unidirectional sensitivity. Vision Research, 42, 1195–1203.
  • Holmes, V. M. (2006). Adult word recognition and visual sequential memory. Reading and Writing, 25, 23–44. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9245-z
  • Holmes, V. M., Malone, A. M., & Renenback, H. (2008). Orthographic processing and visual sequential memory in unexpectedly poor spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 136–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00364.x
  • Holmes, V. M., & Ng, E. (1993). Word-specific knowledge, word-recognition strategies and spelling ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 230–257. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1993.1013
  • Ijzendoorn, M. H. V., & Bus, A. G. (1994). Meta-analytic confirmation of the nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 266–275. doi: 10.2307/747877
  • Iles, J., Walsh, V., & Richardson, A. (2000). Visual search performance in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 6, 163–177. doi: 10.1002/1099-0909(200007/09)6:3<163::AID-DYS150>3.0.CO;2-U
  • Jones, M. W., Holly, P., Branigan, H. P., & Kelly, M. L. (2008). Visual deficits in developmental dyslexia: Relationships between non-linguistic visual tasks and their contribution to components of reading. Dyslexia, 14, 95–115. doi: 10.1002/dys.345
  • Kaufman, N. L. (1980). Review of research on reversal errors. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 55–79. doi: 10.2466/pms.1980.51.1.55
  • Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). PALPA: Psycholinguistic assessment of language processing in aphasia. London: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
  • Keen, A. G., & Lovegrove, W. J. (2000). Transient deficit hypothesis and dyslexia: Examination of whole-parts relationship, retinal sensitivity, and spatial temporal frequencies. Vision Research, 40, 705–715. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00208-4
  • Kevan, A., & Pammer, K. (2009). Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3174–3181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.016
  • Laasonen, M., Service, E., & Virsu, V. (2001). Temporal order and processing acuity of visual, auditory and tactile perception in developmentally dyslexic young adults. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural Neuroscience, 1, 394–410. doi: 10.3758/CABN.1.4.394
  • Lacroix, G. L., Costantinescu, I., Cousineau, D., de Almeida, R. G., Segalowitz, N., & von Grünau, M. (2005). Attentional blink differences between adolescent dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Cognition, 57, 115–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.030
  • Lallier, M., Tainturier, M.-J., Dering, B., Donnadieu, S., Valdois, S., & Thierry, G. (2010). Beavioural and ERP evidence for amodal sluggish attentional shifting in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4125–4135. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.027
  • Lallier, M., Thierry, G., Tainturier, M.-J., Donnadieu, S., Peyrin, C., Bilard, C., & Valdois, S. (2009). Auditory and visual stream segregation in children and adults: An assessment of the amodality assumption of the “sluggish attentional shifting” theory of dyslexia. Brain Research, 1302, 132–147. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.037
  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia. Evidence from German and English children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243–262. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400002058
  • Lassus-Sangosse, D., N'guyen-Morel, M.-A., & Valdois, S. (2008). Sequential or simultaneous visual processing deficit in developmental dyslexia. Vision Research, 48, 979–988. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.01.025
  • Martelli, M., Di Filippo, G., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2009). Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision, 9(4), 1–18. doi: 10.1167/9.4.14
  • Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Pseudoname learning by German-speaking children with dyslexia: Evidence for a phonological learning deficit. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 116–133. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2525
  • Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Benassi, M., Bolzani, R., Facoetti, A., Giovagnoli, S., … Vicari, S. (2010). Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: A comparative study. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 863–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.003
  • Messbauer, V. C. S., & de Jong, P. F. (2003). Word, nonword, and visual paired associate learning in Dutch dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 77–96. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00179-0
  • Metsala, J. L., Stanovich, K. E., & Brown, G. D. A. (1998). Regularity effects and the phonological deficit model of reading disabilities: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 279–293. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.279
  • Moll, K., & Jones, M. (2013). Naming fluency in dyslexic and nondyslexic readers: Differential effects of visual crowding in foveal, parafoveal, and peripheral vision. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2085–2091. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.840852
  • Mundy, I. R., & Carroll, J. M. (2013). Spelling-stress regularity effects are intact in developmental dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(4), 816–828. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.719530
  • Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (2009). A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3737–3753. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0173
  • Pammer, K., Lavis, R., Hansen, P., & Cornelissen, P. L. (2004). Symbol-string sensitivity and children's reading. Brain and Language, 89, 601–610. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.01.009
  • Pammer, K., & Vidyasagar, T. R. (2005). Integration of the visual and auditory networks in dyslexia: A theoretical perspective. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(3), 320–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2005.00272.x
  • Pennington, B (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008
  • Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2012). The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22(6), 420–430. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.04.001
  • Perin, D. (1983). Phonemic segmentation and spelling. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 129–144. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1983.tb01849.x
  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273
  • Peterson, R. L., Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2013). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Testing the predictions of the dual-route and connectionist frameworks. Cognition, 126, 20–38. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.007
  • Ram-Tsur, R., Faust, M., & Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2006). Sequential processing deficits of reading disabled persons is independent of inter-stimulus interval. Vision Research, 46, 3949–396. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.07.001
  • Ram-Tsur, R., Faust, M., & Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2008). Poor performance on serial visual tasks in persons with reading disabilities: Impaired working memory?. Journal of learning disabilities, 41(5), 437–450. doi: 10.1177/0022219408321141
  • Ramus, F., & Ahissar, M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: The difficulties of interpreting poor performance and the importance of normal performance. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 104–122. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.677420
  • Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellone, J. M., & White, S. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple cases study of dyslexics adults. Brain, 126, 841–865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076
  • Roach, N. W., & Hogben, J. H. (2004). Attentional modulation of visual processing in adult dyslexia: A spatial-cuing deficit. Psychological Science, 15, 650–654. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00735.x
  • Roelfsema, P. R., van Ooyen, A., & Watanabe, T. (2010). Perceptual learning rules based on reinforcers and attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.005
  • Romani, C., Di Betta, A. M., Tsouknida, E., & Olson, A. (2008). Lexical and nonexical processing in developmental dyslexia: A case for different resources and different impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(6), 798–830. doi: 10.1080/02643290802347183
  • Romani, C., Galluzzi, C., & Olson, A. C. (2011). Phonological-lexical activation: A lexical component or an output buffer? Evidence from aphasic errors. Cortex, 47, 217–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.004
  • Romani, C., & Stringer, M. (1994). Developmental dyslexia: A problem acquiring orthographic/phonological information in the face of good visual memory and good short-term memory. Brain and Language, 57(3), 482–485.
  • Romani, C., Tsouknida, E., Di Betta, A. M., & Olson, A. (2011). Reduced attentional capacity, but normal processing speed and shifting of attention in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from a serial task. Cortex, 47, 715–733. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.05.008
  • Romani, C., & Ward, J. (1995). Word spelling lists contrasting frequency and regularity. Unpublished testing material.
  • Romani, C., Ward, J., & Olson, A. (1999). Developmental surface dysgraphia: What is the underlying cognitive impairment?. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52(1), 97–128. doi: 10.1080/713755804
  • Schonell, F. J. (1985). Essentials in teaching and spelling. London: Macmillan.
  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 383–404. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90270-6
  • Shovman, M. M., & Ahissar, M. (2006). Isolating the impact of visual perception on dyslexics’ reading ability. Vision Research, 46, 3514–3525. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.011
  • Slaghuis, W. L., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1985). Spatial-frequency-dependent visible persistence and specific reading disability. Brain and Cognition, 4, 219–240. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(85)90071-5
  • Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia-second edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
  • Stein, J. (2003). Visual motion sensitivity and reading. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1785–1793. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00179-9
  • Swanson, H. L., & Hsich, C. J. (2009). Reading disabilities in adults: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 79, 1362–1390. doi: 10.3102/0034654309350931
  • Szmalec, A., Loncke, E., Page, M. P. A., & Duyck, W. (2011). Order or dis- order? Impaired Hebb learning in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 37(5), 1270–1279.
  • Terepocki, M., Kruck, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (2002). The incidence and nature of letter orientation errors in reading disabilitiy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 214–233. doi: 10.1177/002221940203500304
  • Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., & Tainturier, M. J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339–363. doi: 10.1002/dys.284
  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 76–123. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1995.1004
  • Vidyasagar, T., & Pammer, K. (2009). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003
  • Von Karolyi, C., Winner, E., Gray, W., & Sherman, G. F. (2003). Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual-spatial ability. Brain and Language, 85, 427–431. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00052-X
  • Vousden, J. I., Brown, G. D. A., & Harley, T. A. (2000). Serial control of phonology in speech production: A hierarchical model. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 101–175. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2000.0739
  • Ward, J., & Romani, C. (1998). Serial position effects and lexical activation in spelling: Evidence from a single case study. Neurocase, 4, 189–206. doi: 10.1080/13554799808410621
  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (revised ed.). New York: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
  • Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale (revised ed.). New York: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
  • White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U., & Ramus, F. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9(3), 237–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00483.x
  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor reading: A deficit in skill-automatization or a phonological deficit?. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 321–340. doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0204_2
  • Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., F-Xavier, A., & Perry, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107, 151–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.004
  • Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, I., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M., … Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 11455–11459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1205566109