535
Views
35
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Reproducing affective norms with lexical co-occurrence statistics: Predicting valence, arousal, and dominance

&
Pages 1584-1598 | Received 30 Jan 2014, Accepted 23 Jun 2014, Published online: 10 Sep 2014

References

  • Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17, 814–823. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787.x
  • Arora, S., Ge, R., Kannan, R., & Moitra, A. (2012, May). Computing a nonnegative matrix factorization--provably. Proceedings of the 44th symposium on Theory of Computing (pp. 145–162). New York: ACM.
  • Augustine, A. A., Mehl, M. R., & Larsen, R. J. (2011). A positivity bias in written and spoken English and its moderation by personality and gender. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 508–515. doi: 10.1177/1948550611399154
  • Baroni, M., Murphy, B., Barbu, E., & Poesio, M. (2010). Strudel: A corpus-based semantic model based on properties and types. Cognitive Science, 34, 222–254. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01068.x
  • Bestgen, Y. (2002). Détermination de la valence affective de termes dans de grands corpus de texte. In Y. Toussaint et C. Nedellec (Eds.), Actes du Colloque International sur la Fouille de Texte (CIFT) '02 (pp. 81–94). Nancy, France: Inria.
  • Bestgen, Y., & Vincze, N. (2012). Checking and bootstrapping lexical norms by means of word similarity indexes. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 998–1006. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0195-z
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. No. C-1). Gainesville: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2010). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Affective ratings of words and instruction manual (Technical Report C-2). Gainesville: University of Florida.
  • Brants, T., & Franz, A. (2006). Web 1 T 5-gram Version 1. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
  • Brysbaert, M., New, B., & Keuleers, E. (2012). Adding part-of-speech information to the SUBTLEX-US word frequencies. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 991–997. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0190-4
  • Bullinaria, J., & Levy, J. (2007). Extracting semantic representations from word co-occurrence statistics: A computational study. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 510–526. doi: 10.3758/BF03193020
  • Calvo, R. A., & D'Mello, S. (2010). Affect detection: An interdisciplinary review of models, methods, and their applications. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 1, 18–37. doi: 10.1109/T-AFFC.2010.1
  • Church, K. W., & Hanks, P. (1990). Word association norms, mutual information, and lexicography. Computational Linguistics, 16, 22–29.
  • Citron, F. M., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2012). How are affective word ratings related to lexicosemantic properties? Evidence from the Sussex Affective Word List. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 1–19.
  • Cule, E., & De Iorio, M. (2012). A semi-automatic method to guide the choice of ridge parameter in ridge regression. arXiv preprint. arXiv: 1205.0686.
  • Dinu, G., Pham, N., & Baroni, M. (2013). DISSECT: DIStributional SEmantics Composition Toolkit. Proceedings of the System Demonstrations of ACL 2013 (51st Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics) (pp. 31–36). East Stroudsburg, PA: ACL.
  • Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2010). Affective norms for 210 British English and Finnish nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 134–140. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.134
  • Gootjes, L., Coppens, L. C., Zwaan, R. A., Franken, I. H., & Van Strien, J. W. (2011). Effects of recent word exposure on emotion-word Stroop interference: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79, 356–363. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.12.003
  • Gottron, T., Radcke, O., & Pickhardt, R. (2013). On the temporal dynamics of influence on the social semantic web. Semantic Web and Web Science (pp. 75–87). New York: Springer.
  • Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in semantic representation. Psychological review, 114, 211–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
  • Hamann, S., & Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. Neuroreport, 13, 15–19. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00008
  • Hsieh, S., Foxe, D., Leslie, F., Savage, S., Piguet, O., & Hodges, J. R. (2012). Grief and joy: Emotion word comprehension in the dementias. Neuropsychology, 26, 624–630. doi: 10.1037/a0029326
  • Islam, A., & Inkpen, D. (2006). Second order co-occurrence PMI for determining the semantic similarity of words. In Calzolari, N., et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the international conference on language resources and evaluation (LREC 2006), (pp. 1033–1038). Paris, France: European Language Resources Association.
  • Jasmin, D., & Casasanto, D. (2012). The QWERTY Effect: How typing shapes the meanings of words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 499–504. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0229-7
  • Jorge-Botana, G., Leon, J. A., Olmos, R., & Escudero, I. (2010). Latent semantic analysis parameters for essay evaluation using small-scale corpora. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 17, 1–29. doi: 10.1080/09296170903395890
  • Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14–34. doi: 10.1037/a0021446
  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
  • Landis, T. (2006). Emotional words: What's so different from just words?. Cortex, 42(6), 823–830. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70424-6
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248–1263. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00275-3
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.
  • Larsen, R. J., Mercer, K. A., Balota, D. A., & Strube, M. J. (2008). Not all negative words slow down lexical decision and naming speed: Importance of word arousal. Emotion, 8, 445–452. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.445
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 273–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x
  • Louwerse, M., & Connell, L. (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 381–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01157.x
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 51–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2008.01003.x
  • Luce, P. A. (1986). A computational analysis of uniqueness points in auditory word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 39(3), 155–158. doi: 10.3758/BF03212485
  • Manning, C. D., & Schütze, H. (1999). Foundations of statistical natural language processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90005-9
  • Mathews, J. R., & Barch, D. M. (2006). Episodic memory for emotional and non-emotional words in individuals with anhedonia. Psychiatry Research, 143, 121–133. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.030
  • Moors, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., Wanmaker, S., van Schie, K., Van Harmelen, A. L., … & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, dominance, and age of acquisition for 4,300 Dutch words. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 169–177. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0243-8
  • Müller, H. M. (2010). Neurolinguistic findings on the language lexicon: The special role of proper names. The Chinese Journal of Physiology, 53, 351–358. doi: 10.4077/CJP.2010.AMM032
  • Pang, B., & Lee, L. (2008). Opinion mining and sentiment analysis. Foundations and Trends in Information Retrieval, 2, 1–135. doi: 10.1561/1500000011
  • Proverbio, A. M., Lilli, S., Semenza, C., & Zani, A. (2001). ERP indexes of functional differences in brain activation during proper and common names retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 39, 815–827. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00003-3
  • Proverbio, A. M., Mariani, S., Zani, A., & Adorni, R. (2009). How are ‘Barack Obama’ and ‘President Elect’ differentially stored in the brain? An ERP investigation on the processing of proper and common noun pairs. PLoS ONE, 4(9), e7126. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007126
  • Quesada, J. (2007). Creating your own LSA spaces. In T. Landauer, D. McNamara, S. Dennis & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Latent Semantic Analysis: A road to meaning (pp. 71–85). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Recchia, G., & Jones, M. N. (2009). More data trumps smarter algorithms: Comparing pointwise mutual information with latent semantic analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 647–656. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.647
  • Recchia, G., & Louwerse, M. M. (2014). Grounding the ungrounded: Estimating locations of unknown place names from linguistic associations and grounded representations. Retrieved from https://mindmodeling.org/cogsci2014/papers/224/index.html
  • Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (affective norms for English words). Behavior Research Methods, 39, 600–605. doi: 10.3758/BF03193031
  • Rehder, B., Schreiner, M. E., Wolfe, M. B., Laham, D., Landauer, T. K., & Kintsch, W. (1998). Using Latent Semantic Analysis to assess knowledge: Some technical considerations. Discourse Processes, 25(2–3), 337–354. doi: 10.1080/01638539809545031
  • Robinson, M. D., Moeller, S. K., & Ode, S. (2010). Extraversion and reward-related processing: Probing incentive motivation in affective priming tasks. Emotion, 10(5), 615–626. doi: 10.1037/a0019173
  • Rohde, D. (2005). SVDLIBC [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://tedlab.mit.edu/~dr/svdlibc/
  • Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1354
  • Scott, G. G., O'Donnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 80, 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.010
  • Scott, G. G., O'Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2012). Emotion words affect eye fixations during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 783–792.
  • Turney, P. (2001). Mining the Web for Synonyms: PMI-IR versus LSA on TOEFL. In L. De Raedt & P. Flach (Eds.), Proceedings of the 12th European Conference on Machine Learning (pp. 491–502). Berlin: Springer.
  • Turney, P. (2002). Thumbs up or thumbs down? Sentiment orientation applied to unsupervised classification of reviews. In S. Johnson (Ed.), Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 417–424). Philadelphia, PA: ACL.
  • Turney, P., & Littman, M. (2003). Measuring praise and criticism: Inference of semantic orientation from association. ACM Transactions on Information Systems, 21, 315–346. doi: 10.1145/944012.944013
  • Turney, P., & Pantel, P. (2010). From frequency to meaning: Vector space models of semantics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 37, 141–188.
  • Verona, E., Sprague, J., & Sadeh, N. (2012). Inhibitory control and negative emotional processing in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 498–510. doi: 10.1037/a0025308
  • Vincze, N. & Bestgen, Y. (2011). Une procédure automatique pour étendre des normes lexicales par l'analyse des cooccurrences dans des textes. Traitement Automatique des Langues, 52, 191–216.
  • Wang, L., Zhu, Z., Bastiaansen, M., Hagoort, P., & Yang, Y. (2013). Recognizing the emotional valence of names: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 125, 118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.006
  • Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1191–1207. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x
  • Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Yarkoni, T., Balota, D. A., & Yap, M. J. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart's N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 971–979. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.971
  • Yen, H.-L. (2006). Processing of proper names in Mandarin Chinese: A behavioral and neuroimaging study (Ph.D. dissertation). Bielefeld University.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.