345
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Effects of Competing Statistical and Testimonial Evidence in Debates About Science

ORCID Icon &
Pages 353-368 | Received 03 May 2019, Accepted 08 Oct 2020, Published online: 13 Dec 2020

References

  • Achterberg, P., de Koster, W., & van der Waal, J. (2017). A science confidence gap: Education, trust in scientific methods, and trust in scientific institutions in the United States, 2014. Public Understanding of Science, 26(6), 704–720. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662515617367
  • Allen, M., & Preiss, R. W. (1997). Comparing the persuasiveness of narrative and statistical evidence using meta-analysis. Communication Research Reports, 14(2), 125–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824099709388654
  • Bai, H. (2018). Evidence that A Large Amount of Low Quality Responses on MTurk Can Be Detected with Repeated GPS Coordinates. https://www.maxhuibai.com/blog/evidence-that-responses-from-repeating-gps-are-random%0A.
  • Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. Acta Psychologica, 44(3), 211–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(80)90046-3
  • Brown, C. P., Propst, S. M., & Woolley, M. (2004). Report: Helping researchers make the case for science. Science Communication, 25(3), 294–303. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547003262599
  • Conway, B. A., Kenski, K., & Wang, D. (2015). The rise of twitter in the political campaign: Searching for intermedia agenda-setting effects in the presidential primary. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20(4), 363–380. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12124
  • Cvetkovich, G., & Winter, P. L. (2003). Trust and social representations of the management of threatened and endangered species. Environment and Behavior, 35(2), 286–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916502250139
  • Dahlstrom, M. F. (2014). Using narratives and storytelling to communicate science with nonexpert audiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(Supplement_4), 13614–13620. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320645111
  • Dahlstrom, M. F., & Ho, S. S. (2012). Ethical considerations of using narrative to communicate science. Science Communication, 34(5), 592–617. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547012454597
  • De Wit, J. B. F., Das, E., & Vet, R. (2008). What works best: Objective statistics or a personal testimonial? An assessment of the persuasive effects of different types of message evidence on risk perception. Health Psychology, 27(1), 110–115. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.110
  • Detenber, B. H., Ho, S. S., Ong, A. H., & Lim, N. W. B. (2018). Complementary versus competitive framing effects in the context of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. Science Communication, 40(2), 173–198. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018758075
  • Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2016). Preference change in competitive political environments. Annual Review of Political Science, 19(1), 13–31. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-020614-095051
  • Fiske, S. T., & Dupree, C. (2014). Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(Supplement_4), 13593–13597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317505111
  • Gallup. (2019, March 27). 40 Years After Three Mile Island, Americans Split on Nuclear Power. Gallup.Com. https://news.gallup.com/poll/248048/years-three-mile-island-americans-split-nuclear-power.aspx
  • Greene, K., & Brinn, L. S. (2003). Messages influencing college women’s tanning bed use: Statistical versus narrative evidence format and a self-assessment to increase perceived susceptibility. Journal of Health Communication, 8(5), 443–461. https://doi.org/10.1080/713852118
  • Groshek, J., & Clough Groshek, M. (2013). Agenda Trending: Reciprocity and the Predictive capacity of social Networking Sites in Intermedia agenda setting across topics over Time. Media and Communication, 1, 15–27. https://doi.org/10.12924/mac2013.01010015
  • Hart, P. S. (2013). The role of numeracy in moderating the influence of statistics in climate change messages. Public Understanding of Science, 22(7), 785–798. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662513482268
  • Hart, P. S., & Feldman, L. (2018). Would it be better to not talk about climate change? The impact of climate change and air pollution frames on support for regulating power plant emissions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 60, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.08.013
  • Hinnant, A., Subramanian, R., & Young, R. (2016). User comments on climate stories: Impacts of anecdotal vs. Scientific evidence. Climatic Change; Dordrecht, 138(3-4), 411–424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-016-1759-1
  • Hoeken, H., & Hustinx, L. (2009). When is statistical evidence superior to anecdotal evidence in supporting probability claims? The role of argument type. Human Communication Research, 35(4), 491–510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01360.x
  • Hornikx, J. (2005). A review of experimental research on the relative persuasiveness of anecdotal, statistical, causal, and expert evidence. Studies in Communication Sciences, 5, 205–216.
  • Hunt, K. P., & Wald, D. M. (2020). The role of scientific source credibility and goodwill in public skepticism toward GM foods. Environmental Communication, 14(7), 971–986. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2020.1725086
  • Hovland, C., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 635–650.
  • International Agency for Research on Cancer. (2019). Agents Classified by the IARC Monographs, Volumes 1–124. https://monographs.iarc.fr/agents-classified-by-the-iarc/.
  • Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
  • Kata, A. (2012). Anti-vaccine activists, Web 2.0, and the postmodern paradigm—An overview of tactics and tropes used online by the anti-vaccination movement. Vaccine, 30(25), 3778–3789. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2011.11.112
  • Kohring, M., & Matthes, J. (2007). Trust in news media: Development and validation of a multidimensional scale. Communication Research, 34(2), 231–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650206298071
  • Kotcher, J. E., Myers, T. A., Vraga, E. K., Stenhouse, N., & Maibach, E. W. (2017). Does engagement in advocacy hurt the credibility of scientists? Results from a randomized national survey experiment. Environmental Communication, 11(3), 415–429. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2016.1275736
  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
  • Landrum, A. R., Hallman, W. K., & Jamieson, K. H. (2019). Examining the impact of expert voices: Communicating the scientific consensus on genetically-modified organisms. Environmental Communication, 13(1), 51–70. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2018.1502201
  • Males, J., & Van Aelst, P. (2020). Did the blue planet set the agenda for plastic pollution? An explorative study on the influence of a documentary on the public, media and political agendas. Environmental Communication. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2020.1780458
  • McComas, K. A., & Trumbo, C. W. (2001). Source credibility in environmental health - risk controversies: Application of meyer’s credibility index. Risk Analysis, 21(3), 467–480. https://doi.org/10.1111/0272-4332.213126
  • Meyer, P. (1988). Defining and measuring credibility of newspapers: Developing an index. Journalism Quarterly, 65(3), 567–574. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769908806500301
  • Moyer-Gusé, E., Tchernev, J. M., & Walther-Martin, W. (2019). The persuasiveness of a humorous environmental narrative combined with an explicit persuasive appeal. Science Communication, 41(4), 422–441. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547019862553
  • Mullinix, K. J., Leeper, T. J., Druckman, J. N., & Freese, J. (2015). The generalizability of survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 2(2), 109–138. https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2015.19
  • National Science Board. (2016). Science & Engineering Indicators 2016.
  • Nisbet, E. C., Hart, P. S., Myers, T., & Ellithorpe, M. (2013). Attitude change in competitive framing environments? Open-/closed-mindedness, framing effects, and climate change. Journal of Communication, 63(4), 766–785. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12040
  • Nurse, M. S., & Grant, W. J. (2020). I’ll see it when i believe it: Motivated numeracy in perceptions of climate change risk. Environmental Communication, 14(2), 184–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2019.1618364
  • O’Brien, T. L. (2013). Scientific authority in policy contexts: Public attitudes about environmental scientists, medical researchers, and economists. Public Understanding of Science, 22(7), 799–816. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662511435054
  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 69–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.1.69
  • Pew Research Center. (2009). Public praises science; Scientists fault public, Media: Scientific achievements less prominent than a decade ago. https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2009/07/09/public-praises-science-scientists-fault-public-media/.
  • Pew Research Center. (2015). Public and Scientists’ Views on Science and Society. https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2015/01/29/public-and-scientists-views-on-science-and-society/.
  • Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x
  • Porter, T. M. (1996). Trust in numbers: The pursuit of objectivity in science and public Life. Princeton University Press.
  • Rosenthal, S., & Dahlstrom, M. F. (2019). Perceived influence of proenvironmental testimonials. Environmental Communication, 13(2), 222–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1287112.
  • Ross, A. S., & Rivers, D. J. (2019). Internet memes, media frames, and the conflicting logics of climate change discourse. Environmental Communication, 13(7), 975–994. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2018.1560347
  • Shen, L., & Bigsby, E. (2013). The effects of message Features: Content, structure, and Style. In J. P. Dillard, & L. Shen (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of Persuasion: Developments in theory and practice (pp. 20–35). SAGE Publications Ltd.
  • Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (2002). Entertainment-education and elaboration likelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative Persuasion. Communication Theory, 12, 173–191. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2002.tb00265.x
  • Sleeth-Keppler, D., Perkowitz, R., & Speiser, M. (2017). It’s a matter of trust: American judgments of the credibility of informal communicators on solutions to climate change. Environmental Communication, 11(1), 17–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2015.1062790
  • Slovic, S., & Slovic, P. (2015). Numbers and nerves: Information, emotion, and meaning in a World of data. Oregon State University Press.
  • Spoel, P., Goforth, D., Cheu, H., & Pearson, D. (2008). Public communication of climate change science: Engaging citizens through apocalyptic narrative explanation. Technical Communication Quarterly, 18(1), 49–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/10572250802437382
  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x
  • Vraga, E. K., Myers, T., Kotcher, J., Beall, L., & Maibach, E. (2018). Scientific risk communication about controversial issues influences public perceptions of scientists’ political orientations and credibility. Royal Society Open Science, 5(2), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170505
  • Wojcieszak, M., & Kim, N. (2016). How to improve attitudes toward disliked groups: The effects of narrative versus numerical evidence on political Persuasion. Communication Research, 43(6), 785–809. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650215618480
  • Wynne, B. (1992). Misunderstood misunderstanding: Social identities and public uptake of science. Public Understanding of Science, 1(3), 281–304. https://doi.org/10.1088/0963-6625/1/3/004
  • Zebregs, S., van den Putte, B., Neijens, P., & de Graaf, A. (2015). The differential impact of statistical and narrative evidence on beliefs, attitude, and intention: A meta-analysis. Health Communication, 30, 282–289. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2013.842528
  • Zillmann, D. (1999). Exemplification theory: Judging the whole by some of its parts. Media Psychology, 1(1), 69–94. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532785xmep0101

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.