398
Views
44
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Processing Semitic Scripts: Reading and Writing in Arabic and Hebrew

A tale of one letter: Morphological processing in early Arabic spelling

Pages 169-188 | Published online: 29 Nov 2013

REFERENCES

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1995). Learning to read in Arabic: Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Arabic readers. Reading Psychology, 16, 351–394. doi:10.1080/0270271950160401
  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2007). The role of morphology and short vowelization in reading Arabic among normal and dyslexic readers in Grades 3, 6, 9, and 12. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(2), 89–106. doi:10.1007/s10936-006-9035-6
  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16, 423–442. doi:10.1023/A:1024237415143
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Sammour, R. (2013). Spelling errors' analysis of regular and Dyslexic Bilingual Arabic-English Students. Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 3, 58–68.
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and spelling error analysis of native Arabic dyslexic readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17(7–8), 651–690. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2657-x
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2006). Reading in Arabic orthography: Characteristics, research findings, and assessment. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 365–376). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Allaith, Z. A., & Joshi, R. M. (2011). Spelling performance in English consonants among students whose first-language is Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 1089–1110. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9294-3
  • Al-Mannai, H., & Everatt, J. (2005). Phonological processing skills as predictors of literacy amongst Arabic speaking Bahraini children. Dyslexia, 11, 269–291. doi:10.1002/dys.303
  • Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10), 1–165. doi:10.2307/1166112
  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621–635. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000316
  • Azzam, R. (1993). The nature of Arabic reading and spelling errors of young children: A descriptive study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 355–385. doi:10.1007/BF01043112
  • Bakalla, M. H. (2009). tafxi:m. EALL IV. In K. Versteegh (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Arabic Language and Linguistics, (pp. 421–424). I-IV. Leiden: E. J. Brill.
  • Bar-On, A., & Ravid, D. (2011). Morphological analysis in learning to read pseudowords in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 553–581. doi:10.1017/S014271641100021X
  • Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Morphological factors in visual word recognition in Hebrew. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 217–292). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2000). Non-concatenative morphemes in language processing: Evidence from Modern Standard Arabic. Proceedings of the Workshop on Spoken Word Access Processes, 1, 23–26, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2001). Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon. Cognition, 81(1), 65–92. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00119-6
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2004). Allomorphic variation in Arabic:Implications for lexical processing and representation. Brain and Language, 90(1–3) 106–116. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00424-3
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2005). Discontinuous morphology in time: Incremental masked priming in Arabic. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 207–260. doi:10.1080/01690960444000106
  • Boudelaa, S. ( in press). Is the Arabic Mental Lexicon Morpheme-based or stem based? Implications for spoken and written word recognition. In E. Saiegh-Haddad & M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. Springer: The Netherlands.
  • Brittain, M. M. (1970). Inflectional performance and early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 6(1), 34–48. doi:10.2307/747047
  • Broselow, E. (2008). Phonology. EALL IV. In K. Versteegh (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Arabic Language and Linguistics, (pp. 607–615). I-IV. Leiden: E. J. Brill.
  • Brown, R. (1973). A First language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Bybee, J. (1988). Morphology as lexical organization. In M. Hammond & M. Noonan (Eds.), Theoretical morphology: Approaches in modern Linguistics (pp. 119–141). San Diego: Academic Press.
  • Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425–455. doi:10.1080/01690969508407111
  • Caravolas, M., Kessler, B., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Effects of orthographic consistency, frequency, and letter knowledge on children's vowel spelling development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 307–321. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.08.001
  • Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth and eighth grades. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266. doi:10.1017/S0142716400007839
  • Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169–190. doi:10.1023/A:1008131926604
  • Clahsen, H. (1999). Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 991–1060.
  • Daniels, P. T. (1992). The syllabic origin of writing and the segmental origin of the alphabet. In P. Downing, S. D. Lima, & M. Noonan (Eds.), The linguistics of literacy (pp. 83–110). Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
  • Deacon, H. S., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological?” The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001110
  • Derwing, B. L. (1976). Morpheme recognition and the learning of rules in children of differing language backgrounds. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 21, 38–66.
  • Derwing, B., & Baker, W. (1979). Recent research in the acquisition of English morphology. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 209–223). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Dugunoglu, A. Y., & Oney, B. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 1–15. doi:10.1017/S014271640000984X
  • Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy ability and phonological processing skills amongst dyslexic and non-dyslexic speakers of Arabic. Reading & Writing, 20, 273–294. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9031-0
  • Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209–240. doi:10.1007/BF02648177
  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2004). Morphological and orthographic effects on hemispheric processing of nonwords: A cross-linguistic comparison. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 691–705. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2659-8
  • Fayol, M., & Totereau, C. (2001). Learning the written morphology of plural in written French. In L. Tolchinski (Ed.), Developmental aspects in learning to write (pp. 151–173). Doredrecht: Kluwer Academic.
  • Frost, R. (2006). Becoming literate in Hebrew: The grain-size hypothesis and Semitic orthographic systems. Developmental Science, 9, 439–440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00523.x
  • Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263–279. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11001841
  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(1), 104–115. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.104
  • Gillis, S., & Ravid, D.2006. Typological effects on spelling development: A crosslinguistic study of Hebrew and Dutch. Journal of Child Language, 33, 621–659. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007434
  • Gleitman, L., Gleitman, H., Landau, B., & Wanner, E. (1988). Where learning begins. In F. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey (Vol. 111, pp. 150–193). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Grainger, J. (2008). Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction. Language and cognitive processes, 23(1), 1–35. doi:10.1080/01690960701578013
  • Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In P. Bonin (Ed.), The mental lexicon (pp. 1–24). New York, NY: Nova Science.
  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1977). Aims and perspectives in Linguistics. Occasional papers number 1. Applied Linguistics Association of Australia, Center for Language, Learning and Teaching, University of Southern Queensland, Australia.
  • Hansen, G. F. (in press). Word recognition in Arabic: Approaching a Language-Specific Reading Model. In E. Saiegh-Haddad & M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. Springer.
  • Holes, C. (2004). Modern Arabic: Structures, functions and varieties. Revisited Edition. London: Longman.
  • Idrissi, A., Prunet, J. F., & Beland, R. (2008). On the mental representation of Arabic roots. Linguistic Inquiry, 39, 221–259. doi:10.1080/01690960444000214
  • Kager, R. (2009). Stress. In K. Versteegh (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Arabic Language and Linguistics. EALL IV (pp. 344–353). I–IV. Leiden: E. J. Brill.
  • Katz, R. B. (1986). Phonological deficiencies in children with reading disability: Evidence from an object naming task. Cognition, 22, 225–257. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(86)90016-8
  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). Reading in different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 67–84). Amsterdam: Elsevier North Holland.
  • Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children's morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389–410. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5
  • Laudanna, A., & Burani, C. (1995). Distributional properties of derivational affixes: Implications for processing. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 345–364). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Lennox, C., & Siegel, L. S. (1998). Phonological and orthographic processes in good and poor spellers. In M. Joshi & C. Hulme (Eds.). Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 395–404). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Leonard, L. B. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179–202. doi:10.1017/S0142716400008511
  • Leonard, L. (1992). The use of morphology by children with specific language impairment: Evidence from three languages. In R. Chapman (Ed.), Processes in language acquisition and disorders (pp. 186–201). St. Louis: Mosby-Yearbook.
  • Leonard, L., McGregor, K., & Allen, G. (1992). Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1076–1085.
  • Levin, I., Ravid, D., & Rapaport, S. (2001). Morphology and spelling among Hebrew-speaking children: From kindergarten to first grade. Journal of Child Language, 28, 741–769. doi:10.1017/S0305000901004834
  • Mahfoudhi, A. (2007). Roots and patterns in Arabic lexical processing. In E. Benmamoun (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XIX (pp. 123–150). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  • Mahfoudhi, A., Elbeheri, G., Al-Rashidi, M., & Everatt, J. (2010). The role of morphological awareness in reading comprehension among typical and learning disabled native Arabic speakers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 500–514. doi:10.1177/0022219409355478
  • Marcus, G. F. (2000). Children's overregularization and its implications for cognition. In P. Broeder & J. Murre (Eds.), Models of language acquisition: Inductive and deductive approaches (pp. 154–176). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • McClelland, J. L., & Patterson, K. (2002). Rules or connections in past-tenseinflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 465–472. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9
  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Routledge.
  • Muhamed, W., Landerl, K., & Elbert, T. (in press). An Epidemiological survey of specific reading and spelling disorders in Arabic Speaking Children in Egypt. In E. Saiegh-Haddad & M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. The Netherlands: Springer.
  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Spelling and grammar: The necsed move. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Perea, M., Mallouh, R. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). The search of an input coding scheme: Transposed-letter priming in Arabic. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 375–380. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.3.375
  • Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(1), 3–24. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02
  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730
  • Perfetti, C., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization and universals in the development of reading skill: How Chinese research informs a universal science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 5–21. doi:10.1080/10888438.2012.689786
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9, 296–316. doi:10.1080/15475441.2013.813828
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Orthography to phonology and meaning: Comparisons across and within writing systems. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 193–210. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2344-y
  • Perfetti, C. A., Rieben, L., & Fayol, M. (1997). Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages ( Eds.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Perfetti, C., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a :“universal” phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227–248). Amsterdams: North Holland.
  • Prunet, J. F., Beland, R., & Idrissi, A. (2000). The mental representation of Semitic words. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 609–648. doi:10.1017/S0952675799003796
  • Rabin, J., & Deacon, H. (2008). The representation of morphologically complex words in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language, 35, 453–465. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008525
  • Rahbari, N., & Senechal, M. (2009). Lexical and nonlexical processes in the skilled reading and spelling of Persian. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 511–530. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9122-1
  • Ravid, D. (2001). Learning to spell in Hebrew: Phonological and morphological factors. Reading and Writing, 14, 459–485. doi:10.1023/A:1011192806656
  • Ravid, D. D. (2012). Spelling morphology: the psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling. New York, NY: Springer.
  • Ravid, D., & Malenky, A. (2001). Awareness of linear and non-linear morphology in Hebrew: A developmental study. First Language, 21(61), 25–56. doi:10.1177/014272370102106102
  • Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(1), 96–113. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004
  • Russak, S., & Fragman, A. (in press). The development of grapho-phonemic representations among native Hebrew speakers learning Arabic as a foreign language. In E. Saiegh-Haddad & M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. Springer.
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading Acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 431–451. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000225
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 495–512. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001249
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 559–582. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-3180-4
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007). Linguistic constraints on children's ability to isolate phonemes in Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 605–625. doi:10.1017/S0142716407070336
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504. doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hadieh, A., & Ravid, D. (2012). Acquiring noun plurals in Palestinian Arabic: Morphology, familiarity and pattern frequency. Language Learning, 62, 1079–1109. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00727.x
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Henkin-Roitfarb, R. (in press). The structure of Arabic language and orthography. In E. Saiegh-Haddad & M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. Springer.
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., Levin, I., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2011). The Linguistic Affiliation Constraint and phoneme recognition in diglossic Arabic. Journal of Child Language, 38, 297–315. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990365
  • Selby, S. (1972). The development of morphological rules in children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 42, 293–299. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1972.tb00722.x
  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174. doi:10.1348/000712603321661859
  • Shankweiler, D., & Fowler, A. E. (2004). Questions people ask about the role of phonological processes in learning to read. Reading & Writing, 17, 483–515. doi:10.1023/B:READ.0000044598.81628.e6
  • Share, D. L. (2008) On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
  • Slobin, D. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Volume 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 1157–1249). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Snowling, M. J. (1994). Toward a model of spelling acquisition: The development of some components skills. In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 111–128). Toronto: Wiley.
  • Taha, H., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. ( ms. a). Linguistic intervention and spelling development in Arabic. Manuscript submitted for Publication.
  • Taha & Saiegh-Haddad, E. ( ms. b). Morphological transparency and spelling development in Arabic. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Tsesmeli, S. N., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2006). Derivational morphology and spelling in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 587–625. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9011-4
  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649–667.
  • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2007). Cambridge University Versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 913–918. doi:10.3758/BF03194121
  • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2009). Letter transposition effects are not universal: The impact of transposing letters in Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 285–302. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.05.003
  • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2011). Words with and without internal structure: what determines the nurture of orthographic and morphological processing? Cognition, 118, 141–156. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.013
  • Verhoeven, L., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, H. (2003). Units of analysis in reading Dutch bisyllabic pseudowords. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 255–271. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_4
  • Wimmer, H., & Hummer, P. (1990). How German-speaking first graders read and spell: Doubts on the importance of the logographic stage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 349–368. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009620
  • Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (1997). How learning to spell German differs from learning to spell English. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory and practice across languages (pp. 81–96). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16(1/2), 5–20. doi:10.1023/A:1021789804977
  • Zapf, J. A., & Smith, L. B. (2009). Knowing more than one can say: The early regular plural. Journal of Child Language, 36, 1145–1155. doi:10.1017/S0305000909009374
  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Toth, D., Csepe, V., Reis, A., Faisca, L., … Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551–559. doi:10.1177/0956797610363406
  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain & Language, 93, 369–373. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010
  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Orlandi, M., & Spinelli, D. (1999). Markers of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with highgrapheme–phoneme correspondence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 191–216. doi:10.1017/S0142716499002027

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.