Publication Cover
Cognitive Neuroscience
Current Debates, Research & Reports
Volume 10, 2019 - Issue 2
511
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Letter-sound integration in native Chinese speakers learning English: Brain fails in automatic responses but succeeds with more attention

, , , &

References

  • Andres, A. J., Cardy, J. E. O., & Joanisse, M. F. (2011). Congruency of auditory sounds and visual letters modulates mismatch negativity and P300 event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 137–146.
  • Bowden, H. W., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T. (2013). Native-like brain processing of syntax can be attained by university foreign language learners. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2492–2511.
  • Cao, F., Tao, R., Liu, L., Perfetti, C. A., & Booth, J. R. (2013). High proficiency in a second language is characterized by greater involvement of the first language network: Evidence from Chinese learners of English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(10), 1649–1663.
  • Chung, K. K., McBride‐Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Wong, S. W. (2013). General auditory processing, speech perception and phonological awareness skills in Chinese–English biliteracy. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(2), 202–222.
  • Cutting, L. E., & Denckla, M. B. (2001). The relationship of rapid serial naming and word reading in normally developing readers: an exploratory model. Reading and Writing, 14(7–8), 673–705.
  • DeFrancis, J. (1984). The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10(2), 186–202.
  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In: M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–145). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Froyen, D., Bonte, M. L., van Atteveldt, N., & Blomert, L. (2009). The long road to automation: Neurocognitive development of letter–Speech sound processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 567–580.
  • Froyen, D., Van Atteveldt, N., Bonte, M., & Blomert, L. (2008). Cross-modal enhancement of the MMN to speech-sounds indicates early and automatic integration of letters and speech-sounds. Neuroscience Letters, 430(1), 23–28.
  • Froyen, D., Willems, G., & Blomert, L. (2011). Evidence for a specific cross‐modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter–Speech sound processing. Developmental Science, 14(4), 635–648.
  • Greenall, S., & Wen, Q. F. (2008). New standard college English. Beijing: Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press.
  • Hazan, V., Sennema, A., Iba, M., & Faulkner, A. (2005). Effect of audiovisual perceptual training on the perception and production of consonants by Japanese learners of English. Speech Communication, 47(3), 360–378.
  • Ho, C. S. H., & Fong, K. M. (2005). Do Chinese dyslexic children have difficulties learning English as a second language? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(6), 603–618.
  • Horváth, J., Winkler, I., & Bendixen, A. (2008). Do N1/MMN, P3a, and RON form a strongly coupled chain reflecting the three stages of auditory distraction? Biological Psychology, 79(2), 139–147.
  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Shimron, J., & Sparks, R. L. (2005). Predicting foreign language reading achievement in elementary school students. Reading and Writing, 18(6), 527–558.
  • Koda, K. (2007). Reading and language learning: Crosslinguistic constraints on second language reading development. Language Learning, 57(s1), 1–44.
  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 153–169.
  • Luck, S. J. (2005). Event-related potentials: A methods handbook, New York: MIT Press.
  • Mittag, M., Alho, K., Takegata, R., Makkonen, T., & Kujala, T. (2013). Audiovisual attention boosts letter‐speech sound integration. Psychophysiology, 50(10), 1034–1044.
  • Näätänen, R. (1992). Attention and brain function. Erlbaum: Psychology Press.
  • Näätänen, R., & Alho, K. (1995). Mismatch negativity-a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. International Journal of Neuroscience, 80(1–4), 317–337.
  • Nelson, J. R., Liu, Y., Fiez, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2009). Assimilation and accommodation patterns in ventral occipitotemporal cortex in learning a second writing system. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 810–820.
  • Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R. (2005). An ERP study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1212–1228.
  • Pan, J., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, H. (2011). What is in the naming? A 5-year longitudinal study of early rapid naming and phonological sensitivity in relation to subsequent reading skills in both native Chinese and English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 897–908.
  • Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2005). The neural basis of first and second language processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 202–206.
  • Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., … Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 121(10), 1841–1852.
  • Picton, T. W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard, S. A., Johnson, R., … Taylor, M. J. (2000). Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: Recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 127–152.
  • Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128–2148.
  • Rinne, T., Särkkä, A., Degerman, A., Schröger, E., & Alho, K. (2006). Two separate mechanisms underlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention. Brain Research, 1077(1), 135–143.
  • Schröger, E. (1998). Measurement and interpretation of the mismatch negativity. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30(1), 131–145.
  • Snowling, M., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia—A developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3(3), 309–339.
  • Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2012). Do L1 reading achievement and L1 print exposure contribute to the prediction of L2 proficiency? Language Learning, 62(2), 473–505.
  • Stekelenburg, J. J., Keetels, M., & Vroomen, J. (2018). Multisensory integration of speech sounds with letters vs. visual speech: Only visual speech induces the mismatch negativity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(9), 1135–1145.
  • Sussman, E., Winkler, I., & Schröger, E. (2003). Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 630–637.
  • Talsma, D., Doty, T. J., Strowd, R., & Woldorff, M. G. (2006). Attentional capacity for processing concurrent stimuli is larger across sensory modalities than within a modality. Psychophysiology, 43(6), 541–549.
  • Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Feng, C. M., Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Xiong, J., … Gao, J. H. (2003). Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language. Human Brain Mapping, 18(3), 158–166.
  • Wang, M., Koda, K., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Alphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: A comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition, 87(2), 129–149.
  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97(1), 67–88.
  • Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Villringer, A., & Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37(1), 159–170.
  • Weber-Fox, C. M., & Neville, H. J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(3), 231–256.
  • Wei, R., & Su, J. (2012). The statistics of English in China: An analysis of the best available data from government sources. English Today, 28(3), 10–14.
  • Winkler, I., Tervaniemi, M., Schröger, E., Wolff, C., & Näätänen, R. (1998). Preattentive processing of auditory spatial information in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 242(1), 49–52.
  • Wolf, M., O'rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia.Reading and Writing, 15(1–2), 43-72.
  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests, revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
  • Xu, M., Baldauf, D., Chang, C. Q., Desimone, R., & Tan, L. H. (2017). Distinct distributed patterns of neural activity are associated with two languages in the bilingual brain. Science Advances, 3(7), e1603309.
  • Yang, Z., Wang, C., Feng, L., Li, S., & Tao, S. (2016). Unsuccessful letter–Sound integration in English reading by native Chinese speakers: Evidence from an event related potentials study. Science Bulletin, 61(24), 1855–1864.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.