245
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The Contribution of Linguistic and Cognitive Measures to Listening Comprehension among Arabic-speaking Kindergartners

References

  • Adams, A. M., Bourke, L., & Willis, C. (1999). Working memory and spoken language comprehension in young children. International Journal of Psychology, 34(5–6), 364–373. doi:10.1080/002075999399701
  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 19(9), 933–958. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9024-z
  • Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1982). Reading comprehension and the assessment and acquisition of word knowledge. Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report; no. 249.
  • Asadi, I. A, & Abu-Rabia, S. (2019). The impact of the position of phonemes and lexical status on phonological awareness in the diglossic arabic language. Journal Of Psycholinguistic Research. doi: 10.1007/s10936-019-09646-x
  • Asadi, I. A., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). The influence of diglossia on different types of phonological abilities in Arabic. Journal of Education and Learning, 3, 3. doi:10.5539/jel.v3n3p45
  • Asadi, I. A., & Ibrahim, R. (2018). The SVR between transparent and deep versions of the Arabic orthography. Journal of Reading Psychology, 39(6), 1–16. doi:10.1080/02702711.2018.1481477
  • Asadi, I. A, Khateb, A, Ibrahim, R, & Taha, (2017). How do different cognitive and linguistic factors contribute to reading in arabic? a large scale developmental. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(9), 1835-1867. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-9755-z
  • Asadi, I. A., Khateb, A., & Shany, M. (2017). How simple is reading in Arabic? A cross-sectional investigation of reading comprehension from first to sixth grade. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(S1), S1–S22. doi:10.1111/1467-9817.12093
  • Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M. W., & Anderson, M. C. (2009). Memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. doi:10.1038/nrn1201
  • Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2010). Listening comprehension, oral expression, reading comprehension, and written expression: Related yet unique language systems in grades 1, 3, 5, and 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 635. doi:10.1037/a0019319
  • Bishop, D. (1989). Test for reception of grammar. Manchester: University of Manchester.
  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1982). Test for reception of grammar. Manchester:University of Manchester.
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2011). Productivity and priming: Morphemic decomposition in Arabic. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4–6), 624–652. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.521022
  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683–696. doi:10.1348/000709905X67610
  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31
  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 12(3), 169–190. doi:10.1023/A:1008131926604
  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adolf, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts (Ed.), The connection between language and reading disabilities (pp. 23–36). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
  • Cheng, J., & Matthews, J. (2018). The relationship between three measures of L2 vocabulary knowledge and L2 listening and reading. Language Testing, 35(1), 3–25. doi:10.1177/0265532216676851
  • Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1106–1116. doi:10.1177/0956797610375449
  • Denckla, M. B, & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Naming of object-drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain and Language, 3(1), 1-15. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(76)90001-8
  • Diakidoy, I. A. N., Mouskounti, T., & Ioannides, C. (2011). Comprehension and learning from refutation and expository texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(1), 22–38. doi:10.1598/RRQ.46.1.2
  • Dufva, M., Niemi, P., & Voeten, M. J. (2001). The role of phonological memory, word recognition, and comprehension skills in reading development: From preschool to grade 2. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 14(1–2), 91–117. doi:10.1023/A:1008186801932
  • Elbeheri, G., Everatt, J., Mahfoudhi, A., Abu Al-Diyar, M., & Taibah, N. (2011). Orthographic processing and reading comprehension among Arabic speaking mainstream and LD children. Dyslexia, 17(2), 123–142. doi:10.1002/dys.430
  • Farvardin, M. T., & Valipouri, L. (2017). Probing the relationship between vocabulary knowledge and listening comprehension of Iranian lower-intermediate EFL learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature, 6(5), 273–278. doi:10.7575/aiac.ijalel.v.6n.5p.273
  • Ferguson, C.A. (1959). Languages and Social Context, 232-251.
  • Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoè, G., & Levorato, M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(4), 935–951. doi:10.1348/026151008X397189
  • Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Processes, 48(2), 119–138. doi:10.1080/0163853X.2010.494244
  • Fong, C. Y. C., & Ho, C. S. H. (2017). What are the contributing cognitive-linguistic skills for early Chinese listening comprehension? Learning and Individual Differences, 59, 78–85. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2017.08.001
  • Foorman, B. R., Herrera, S., Petscher, Y., Mitchell, A., & Truckenmiller, A. (2015). The structure of oral language and reading and their relation to comprehension in Kindergarten through Grade 2. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 28(5), 655–681. doi:10.1007/s11145-015-9544-5
  • Fracasso, L. E., Bangs, K., & Binder, K. S. (2016). The contributions of phonological and morphological awareness to literacy skills in the adult basic education population. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(2), 140–151. doi:10.1177/0022219414538513
  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177
  • Gottardo, A., Mirza, A., Koh, P. W., Ferreira, A., & Javier, C. (2018). Unpacking listening comprehension: The role of vocabulary, morphological awareness, and syntactic knowledge in reading comprehension. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(8), 1741–1764. doi:10.1007/s1114
  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10. doi:10.1177/074193258600700104
  • Harlaar, N., Cutting, L., Deater-Deckard, K., DeThorne, L. S., Justice, L. M., Schatschneider, C., … Petrill, S. A. (2010). Predicting individual differences in reading comprehension: A twin study. Annals of Dyslexia, 60(2), 265–288. doi:10.1007/s11881-010-0044-7
  • Hogan, T. P., Adlof, S. M., & Alonzo, C. N. (2014). On the importance of listening comprehension. International Journal of Speech-language Pathology, 16(3), 199–207. doi:10.3109/17549507.2014.904441
  • Holes, C. (2004). Modern arabic: structures, functions, and varieties. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160. doi:10.1007/BF00401799
  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(1), 51–82. doi:10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5
  • Katzir, T., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 51–82. doi:10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5
  • Kendeou, P., Bohn‐Gettler, C., White, M. J., & Van Den Broek, P. (2008). Children’s inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 259–272. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.00370.x
  • Kieffer, M. J., Petscher, Y., Proctor, C. P., & Silverman, R. D. (2016). Is the whole greater than the sum of its parts? Modeling the contributions of language comprehension skills to reading comprehension in the upper elementary grades. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(6), 436–454. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000920
  • Kim, Y. S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.003
  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163
  • Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42(2), 75–82. doi:10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x
  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 134–147. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134
  • Oakhill, J., Hartt, J., & Samols, D. (2005). Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 18(7–9), 657–686. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z
  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(4), 443–468. doi:10.1080/01690960344000008
  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730
  • PIRLS. (2003). International report: IEA’s study of reading literacy achievement in primary schools in 2001. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
  • Pizzioli, F., & Schelstraete, M. A. (2013). Real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment: The contribution of lexicosemantic, syntactic, and world-knowledge information. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(1), 181–210. doi:10.1017/S014271641100066X
  • Poskiparta, E., Niemi, P., & Vauras, M. (1999). Who benefits from training in linguistic awareness in the first grade, and what components show training effects? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 437–446. doi:10.1177/002221949903200509
  • Primor, L, Pierce, M. E, & Katzir, T. (2011). Predicting reading comprehension of narrative and expository texts among hebrewspeaking readers with and without a reading disability. Annals Of Dyslexia, 61(2), 242-268. doi:10.1007/s11881-011-0059-8
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(3), 431–451. doi:10.1017/s0142716403000225
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(4), 495–512. doi:10.1017/s0142716404001249
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007). Linguistic constraints children’s ability to isolate phonemes in diglossic Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(4), 607–625. doi:10.1017/S0142716407070336
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Joshi, M. (2014). Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives. New York, London: Springer -Dordrecht Heidlberg.
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., Levin, I., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2011). The linguistic affiliation constraint and phoneme recognition in diglossic Arabic. Journal of Child Language, 38(2), 297–315. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990365
  • Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 2, 173–182.
  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
  • Share, D. L., & Leikin, M. (2004). Language impairment at school entry and later reading disability: Connections at lexical versus supralexical levels of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 87–110. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_5
  • Shatil, E. (1995). Predicting reading ability: Evidence for cognitive modularity. (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of Haifa, Haifa,Israel.
  • Silva, M., & Cain, K. (2015). The relations between lower and higher level comprehension skills and their role in prediction of early reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(2), 321. doi:10.1037/a0037769
  • Smith-Spark, J. H., & Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15(1), 34–56. doi:10.1080/09658210601043384
  • Stahl, S. A. (2003). Vocabulary and readability: How knowing word meanings affects comprehension. Topics in Language Disorders, 23(3), 241–247. doi:10.1097/00011363-200307000-00009
  • Strasser, K., & Del Río, F. (2014). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(2), 169–187. doi:10.1002/rrq.68
  • Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., Van Den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(4), 383–401. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01401.x
  • Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 26(3), 403–429. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9374-7
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1(2), 161–185. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0102_4
  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 3–32. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1101_2
  • Wong, S. W., Mok, P. P., Chung, K. K. H., Leung, V. W., Bishop, D. V., & Chow, B. W. Y. (2017). Perception of native English reduced forms in Chinese learners: Its role in listening comprehension and its phonological correlates. TESOL Quarterly, 51(1), 7–31. doi:10.1002/tesq.273
  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Toth, D., Csepe, V., Reis, A., Faisca, L., … Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551–559. doi:10.1177/0956797610363406

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.