2,166
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Basic Research Article

Investigating the effect of proactive interference control training on intrusive memories

Investigando el efecto del entrenamiento de control de interferencias proactivas en las memorias intrusivas

研究主动干扰控制训练对闯入性记忆的影响

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, , & show all
Article: 1611092 | Received 16 Dec 2018, Accepted 10 Apr 2019, Published online: 14 May 2019

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory-II manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Blackwell, S. E., Woud, M. L., & MacLeod, C. (2017). A question of control? Examining the role of control conditions in experimental psychopathology using the example of cognitive bias modification research. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20, e54.
  • Bomyea, J., & Amir, N. (2011). The effect of an executive functioning training program on working memory capacity and intrusive thoughts. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(6), 529–12.
  • Bomyea, J., Amir, N., & Lang, A. J. (2012). The relationship between cognitive control and posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(2), 844–848.
  • Bomyea, J., Stein, M. B., & Lang, A. J. (2015). Interference control training for PTSD: A randomized controlled trial of a novel computer-based intervention. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 34, 33–42.
  • Brewin, C. R., & Smart, L. (2005). Working memory capacity and suppression of intrusive thoughts. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36, 61–68.
  • Bunting, M. (2006). Proactive interference and item similarity in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 183–196.
  • Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365.
  • Calamia, M., Markon, K., & Tranel, D. (2012). Scoring higher the second time around: Meta- analysis of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment. The Clinical Neuropsychology, 276, 543e570.
  • Calero, M. D., & Navarro, E. (2007). Cognitive plasticity as a modulating variable on the effects of memory training in elderly persons. Archives of Clinical Neuro-Psychology, 22, 63e72.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Dalgleish, T. (2004). Cognitive approaches to posttraumatic stress disorder: The evolution of multirepresentational theorizing. Psychological Bulletin, 130(2), 228–260.
  • Duff, K., Beglinger, L. J., Schultz, S. K., Moser, D. J., McCaffrey, R. J., Haase, R. F., … Paulsen, J. S. (2007). Practice effects in the prediction of long-term cognitive outcome in three patient samples: A novel prognostic index. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(1), 15–24. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.08.013.
  • Duff, K., Beglinger, L. J., Moser, D. J., Schultz, S. K., & Paulsen, J. S. (2010). Practice effects and outcome of cognitive training: Preliminary evidence from a memory training course. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 91.
  • Engle, R. W. (2001). What is working memory capacity? In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Suprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 297–314). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23.
  • Görgen, S. M., Hiller, W., & Witthöft, M. (2016). Die Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS) - Entwicklung und teststatistische Prüfung einer deutschen Adaption. Diagnostica, 62(1), 31–43.
  • Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., & Kühner, C. (2006). Beck Depressionsinventar II. Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI II. Frankfurt a. M.: Harcourt Test Services.
  • Heeren, A., Coussement, C., & McNally, R. J. (2016). Untangling attention bias modification from emotion: A double-blind randomized experiment among individuals with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 61–67.
  • Holmes, E. A., & Steel, C. (2004). Schizotypy: A vulnerability factor for traumatic intrusions. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192(1), 28–34.
  • James, E. L., Bonsall, M. B., Hoppitt, L., Tunbridge, E. M., Geddes, J. R., Milton, A. L., & Holmes, E. A. (2015). Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation-update mechanisms. Psychological Science, 1–15. doi:10.1177/0956797615583071
  • James, E. L., Lau-Zhu, A., Clark, I. A., Visser, R. M., Hagenaars, M. A., & Holmes, E. A. (2016). The trauma film paradigm as an experimental psychopathology model of psychological trauma: Intrusive memories and beyond. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 106–142.
  • JASP team. (2018). JASP (version 0.9). Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(2), 336–358.
  • Kessler, H., Schmidt, A.-C., James, E., Blackwell, S. E., von Rauchhaupt, M., Harren, K., … Holmes, E. A. (2019). Visuospatial computer game play after memory reactivation delivered three days after a traumatic film reduces the number of intrusive memories of the experimental trauma. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.01.006
  • Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(5), 565–579.
  • Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
  • Lustig, C., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 130(2), 199–207.
  • Maercker, A., & Schützwohl, M. (1998). Erfassung von psychischen belastungsfolgen: Die Impact of Event Skala-revidierte Version (IES-R). [Assessment of post-traumatic stress reactions: The Impact of Event Scale-Revised (IES-R).]. Diagnostica, 44, 130–141.
  • Meyer, T., Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Flashbacks, intrusions, mind-wandering – Instances of an involuntary memory spectrum: A commentary on Takarangi, Strange, and Lindsay (2014). Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 24–29.
  • Nixon, R. D., Nehmy, T., & Seymour, M. (2007). The effect of cognitive load and hyperarousal on negative intrusive memories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2652–2663.
  • Reisberg, D., Pearson, D. G., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2003). Intuitions and introspections about imagery: The role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator´s theoretical views. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 147–160.
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  • Takarangi, M. K., Strange, D., & Lindsay, D. S. (2014). Self-report may underestimate trauma intrusions. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 297–305.
  • Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 498–505.
  • Vasterling, J. J., & Hall, K. A. A. (2018). Neurocognitive and information processing biases in posttraumatic stress disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(11), 99.
  • Verwoerd, J., de Jong, P. J., & Wessel, I. (2008). Low attentional control and the development of inrusive memories following a laboratory stressor. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 30, 291–297.
  • Verwoerd, J., Wessel, I., & de Jong, P. J. (2009). Individual differences in experiencing intrusive memories: The role of the ability to resist proactive interference. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(2), 189–201.
  • Verwoerd, J., Wessel, I., de Jong, P. J., Nieuwenhuis, M. M., & Huntjens, R. J. (2011). Pre-stressor interference control and intrusive memories. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(2), 161–170.
  • Voss, M., Ehring, T., & Wolkenstein, L. (2018). Does transcranial direct current stimulation affect post-stressor intrusive memories and rumination? An experimental analogue study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1–1.
  • Weiss, D., & Marmar, C. (1996). The impact of event scale - revised. In J. Wilson & T. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD (pp. 399–411). New York: Guilford.
  • Wessel, I., Huntjens, R. J., & Verwoerd, J. R. (2010). Cognitive control and suppression of memories of an emotional film. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(2), 83–89.
  • Woud, M. L., Cwik, J. C., Kleim, B., Blackwell, S. E., Holmes, E. A., Adolph, D., … Margraf, J. (2018). Does napping enhance the effects of Cognitive Bias Modification Appraisal training? An experimental study. PloS one, 13, e0192837.
  • Woud, M. L., Holmes, E. A., Postma, P., Dalgleish, T., & Mackintosh, B. (2012). Ameliorating intrusive memories of distressing experiences using computerized reappraisal training. Emotion, 12, 778–784.
  • Woud, M. L., Verwoerd, J., & Krans, J. (2017). Modification of cognitive biases related to posttraumatic stress: A systematic review and research agenda. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 81–95.
  • Zlomuzica, A., Woud, M. L., Machulska, A., Kleimt, K., Dietrich, L., Wolf, O. T., … Margraf, J. (2018). Deficits in episodic memory and mental time travel in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 83, 42–54.