240
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Time for action versus action in time: time estimation differs between motor preparation and execution

, &
Pages 129-136 | Received 02 Apr 2016, Accepted 30 Aug 2016, Published online: 16 Sep 2016

References

  • Allan, L. G. (1979). The perception of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 26(5), 340–354. doi:10.3758/BF03204158
  • Andersen, R. A., & Buneo, C. A. (2002). Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 189–220. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142922
  • Block, R. A. (1989). A contextualistic view of time and mind. In J. T. Fraser (Ed.), Time and mind: Interdisciplinary issues (pp. 61–79). Madison, WI: Intl Universities Pr.
  • Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 330–343. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.006
  • Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1996). Models of psychological time revisited. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind (pp. 171–195). Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
  • Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2001). Retrospective and prospective timing: Memory, attention, and consciousness. In C. M. Hoerl (Ed.), Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 59–76). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Brown, S. W. (1985). Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception & Psychophysics, 38(2), 115–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03198848
  • Brown, S. W. (1997). Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(7), 1118–1140. doi: 10.3758/BF03205526
  • Eagleman, D. M., & Pariyadath, V. (2009). Is subjective duration a signature of coding efficiency? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1841–1851. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0026
  • Fraisse, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In M. P. Carterette & E. C. Friedmans (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol. 8, pp. 203–254). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Freeman, J. B., Ambady, N., Midgley, K. J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2011). The real-time link between person perception and action: Brain potential evidence for dynamic continuity. Social Neuroscience, 6(2), 139–155. doi:10.1080/17470919.2010.490674
  • Grondin, S. (1998). Judgments of the duration of visually marked empty time intervals: Linking perceived duration and sensitivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 60(2), 319–330. doi: 10.3758/BF03206040
  • Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5(4), 382–385. doi:10.1038/nn827
  • Hagura, N., Kanai, R., Orgs, G., & Haggard, P. (2012). Ready steady slow: Action preparation slows the subjective passage of time. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1746), 4399–4406. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1339
  • Hunt, A. R., Chapman, C. S., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Taking a long look at action and time perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(1), 125–136. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.125
  • Ivry, R. B. (1996). The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(6), 851–857. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80037-7
  • Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Torriero, S., Salerno, S., Lo Gerfo, E., & Caltagirone, C. (2007). Repetitive TMS of cerebellum interferes with millisecond time processing. Experimental Brain Research, 179(2), 291–299. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0791-1
  • Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2009). Vierordt’s The Experimental Study of the Time Sense (1868) and its legacy. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(6), 941–960. doi: 10.1080/09541440802453006
  • Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1583–1592. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00118-0
  • Michaels, C. F., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Mioni, G., Stablum, F., McClintock, S. M., & Grondin, S. (2014). Different methods for reproducing time, different results. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(3), 675–681. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0625-3
  • Morrone, M. C., Ross, J., & Burr, D. (2005). Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space. Nature Neuroscience, 8(7), 950–954. doi:10.1038/nn1488
  • Rammsayer, T. H., & Lima, S. D. (1991). Duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals: Cognitive and perceptual factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 50(6), 565–574. doi: 10.3758/BF03207541
  • Schubotz, R. I., Friederici, A. D., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2000). Time perception and motor timing: A common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage, 11(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0514
  • Shibasaki, H., & Hallett, M. (2006). What is the Bereitschaftspotential? Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(11), 2341–2356. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.04.025
  • Thomas, E. A., & Weaver, W. B. (1975). Cognitive processing and time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 17(4), 363–367. doi:10.3758/BF03199347
  • Toplak, M. E., Rucklidge, J. J., Hetherington, R., John, S. C., & Tannock, R. (2003). Time perception deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbid reading difficulties in child and adolescent samples. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 888–903. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00173
  • Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1–31. doi: 10.1037/h0093864
  • Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., & Naish, P. L. (1992). On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 45(2), 235–263. doi: 10.1080/14640749208401326
  • Wearden, J. H. (2005). Origins and development of internal clock theories of psychological time. Psychologie Française, 50, 7–25. doi:10.1.1.116.1006
  • Wearden, J. H., Norton, R., Martin, S., & Montford-Bebb, O. (2007). Internal clock processes and the filled-duration illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 716–729.
  • Wittmann, M. (1999). Time perception and temporal processing levels of the brain. Chronobiology International, 16(1), 17–32. doi: 10.3109/07420529908998709
  • Yarrow, K., Haggard, P., Heal, R., Brown, P., & Rothwell, J. C. (2001). Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity. Nature, 414(6861), 302–305. doi:10.1038/35104551
  • Zakay, D. (1990). The evasive art of subjective time measurement: Some methodological dilemmas.
  • Zakay, D. (1993). Time estimation methods – do they influence prospective duration estimates? Perception, 22(1), 91–101. doi: 10.1068/p220091
  • Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 12–16. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512604

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.