315
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Spatial reference frames and SNARCFootnote

&
Pages 113-128 | Received 25 Mar 2016, Accepted 10 Oct 2016, Published online: 06 Nov 2016

References

  • Ansorge, U., & Wuhr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.
  • Bächtold, D., Baumüller, M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36, 731–735. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00002-5
  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
  • Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641.
  • Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial representation of numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 43–54). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2010). How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial-numerical associations. Brain and Cognition, 72, 333–336. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010
  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition—from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 67, 1461–1483. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.927515
  • Fischer, M. H., Shaki, S., & Cruise, A. (2009). It takes just one word to quash a SNARC. Experimental Psychology, 56, 361–366. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.5.361
  • Gevers, W., & Lammertyn, J. (2005). The hunt for the SNARC. Psychology Science, 47, 10–21.
  • Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, 122, 221–233. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004
  • Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2010). Verbal-Spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 180–190. doi: 10.1037/a0017688
  • Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44.
  • Hartmann, M., Gashaj, V., Stahnke, A., & Mast, F. W. (2014). There is more than “more is up”: Hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1401–1414.
  • Holmes, K. J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Orienting numbers in mental space: Horizontal organization trumps vertical. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 65, 1044–1051. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.685079
  • Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in the parietal cortex. Nature Review Neuroscience, 6, 435–448. doi: 10.1038/nrn1684
  • Hung, Y.-H., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Wu, D. H. (2008). Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers. Cognition, 106, 1441–1450. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.017
  • Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 662–673. doi: 10.3758/BF03195857
  • Lorch, R. F. & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 149–157.
  • Müller, D., & Schwarz, W. (2007). Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1151–1161. doi: 10.3758/BF03193485
  • Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416
  • Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 32–45. doi: 10.1037/h0028566
  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. l., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t-tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225
  • Santens, S., & Gevers, W. (2008). The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line. Cognition, 108, 263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.01.002
  • Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with manual and saccadic responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 651–664. doi: 10.3758/BF03194909
  • Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 328–331. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.328
  • Viarouge, A., Hubbard, E. M., & Dehaene, S. (2014). The organization of spatial reference frames involved in the SNARC effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 67, 1484–1499. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.897358
  • Winter, B., Matlock, T., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Mental number space in three dimensions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 209–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.005
  • Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006a). Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: A failure to replicate Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42, 1069–1079. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70219-3
  • Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006b). Variability of the SNARC effect: Systematic interindividual differences or just random error? Cortex, 42, 1119–1123. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70223-5
  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50, 489–525.
  • Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 166–190. doi: 10.1163/1568537054068660

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.