385
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The influence of high autistic personality traits on the attribution of intentionality in typically developing individuals

, , &
Pages 840-853 | Received 03 Nov 2017, Accepted 12 Sep 2018, Published online: 04 Oct 2018

References

  • Aaron, R. V., Benson, T. L., & Park, S. (2015). Investigating the role of alexithymia on the empathic deficits found in schizotypy and autism spectrum traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 77, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.032
  • Abell, F., Happé, F. G., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00014-9
  • Adams, F., & Steadman, A. (2004). Intentional action in ordinary language: Core concept or pragmatic understanding. Analysis, 64, 173–181. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8284.2004.00480.x doi: 10.1093/analys/64.2.173
  • Alfano, M., Beebe, J., & Brian, R. (2012). The centrality of belief and reflection in knobe-effect cases. The Monist, 95, 264–289. doi: 10.5840/monist201295215
  • Alicke, M. D. (2008). Blaming badly. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 179–186. doi: 10.1163/156770908X289279
  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Ed. it. Massimo Biondi (a cura di), DSM-5. Manuale diagnostico e statistico dei disturbi mentali. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore. ISBN 978-88-6030-661-6.
  • Baez, S., Couto, B., Torralva, T., Sposato, L. A., Huepe, D., Montañes, P., … Ibanez, A. (2014). Comparing moral judgments of patients with frontotemporal dementia and frontal stroke. JAMA Neurology, 71(9), 1172–1176. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.347
  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient (EQ): An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175. doi:10.1023/A:1005653411471 doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471
  • Bensi, L., Giusberti, F., Nori, R., & Gambetti, E. (2010). Individual differences and reasoning: A study on personality traits. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 545–562. doi: 10.1348/000712609X471030
  • Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2013). Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry, 3, e285. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.61
  • Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 133, 1515–1525. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq060
  • Blair, R. (1999). Psychophysiological responsiveness to the distress of others in children with autism. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 477–485. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00154-8
  • Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1(3), 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301
  • Buon, M., Dupoux, E., Jacob, P., Chaste, P., Leboyer, M., & Zalla, T. (2013). The role of causal and intentional judgments in moral reasoning in individuals with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 458–470. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1588-7
  • Cokely, E. T., & Feltz, A. (2009). Individual differences, judgment biases, and theory-of-mind: Deconstructing the intentional action side effect asymmetry. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.10.007
  • Constantino, J. N. (2011). The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatric Research, 69, 55–62. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212ec6e
  • Corcoran, R., Cahill, C., & Frith, C. D. (1997). The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: A study of “mentalizing” ability. Schizophrenia Research, 24, 319–327. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00117-X
  • Cova, F. (2016). The folk concept of intentional action: Empirical approaches. In W. Buckwalter & J. Sytsma (Eds.), Blackwell companion to experimental philosophy. doi: 10.1002/9781118661666
  • Cova, F., Lantian, A., & Boudesseul, J. (2016). Can the knobe effect be explained away? Methodological controversies in the study of the relationship between intentionality and morality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 10. doi: 10.1177/0146167216656356
  • Cushman, F., & Mele, A. (2008). Intentional action: Two and half folk concepts. In J. Knobe, & S. Nichols (Eds.), Experimental philosophy (pp. 170–184). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127, 6–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008
  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
  • Dodell-Feder, D., Lincoln, S. H., Coulson, J. P., & Hooker, C. I. (2013). Using fiction to assess mental state understanding: A New task for assessing theory of mind in adults. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e81279. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081279
  • Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., … Convit, A. (2006). Introducing MASC: A movie for the assessment of social cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0
  • Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Covit, A. (2008). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 464. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x
  • Fan, Y. T., Chen, C. Y., Chen, C. H., Decety, J., & Cheng, Y. W. (2014). Empathic arousal and social understanding in individuals with autism: Evidence from fMRI and ERP measurements. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1203–1213. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst101
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Feltz, A., & Cokely, E. T. (2011). Individual differences in theory-of-mind judgments: Order effects and side effects. Philosophical Psychology, 24(3), 343–355. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2011.556611
  • Fu, G., Xiao, W., Killen, M., & Lee, K. (2014). Moral judgment and its relation to second-order theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2085–2092. doi: 10.1037/a0037077
  • Gillespie, S. M., Mitchell, J. I., & Abu-Akel, A. M. (2017). Autistic traits and positive psychotic experiences modulate the association of psychopathic tendencies with theory of mind in opposite directions. Scientific Reports, 7, 6485. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06995-2
  • Gökçen, E., Frederickson, N., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). Theory of mind and executive control deficits in typically developing adults and adolescents with high levels of autism traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2072–2087. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2735-3
  • Gökçen, E., Petrides, K. V., Hudry, K., Frederickson, N., & Smillie, L. D. (2014). Sub-threshold autism traits: The role of trait emotional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 187–199. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12033
  • Gu, X., Eilam-Stock, T., Zhou, T., Anagnostou, E., Kolevzon, A., Soorya, L., … Fan, J. (2015). Autonomic and brain responses associated with empathy deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 3323–3338. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22840
  • Gvozdic, K., Moutier, S., Dupoux, E., & Buon, M. (2016). Priming children’s use of intentions in moral judgement with metacognitive training. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00190
  • Hadjikhani, N., Zürcher, N. R., Rogier, O., Hippolyte, L., Lemonnier, E., Ruest, T., … Prkachin, K. M. (2014). Emotional contagion for pain is intact in autism spectrum disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 4(1), e343. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.113
  • Hindriks, F. (2008). Intentional action and the praise-blame asymmetry. The Philosophical Quarterly, 58, 630–641. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9213.2007.551.x
  • Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0538-x
  • Jones, A. P., Happé, F. G., Gilbert, F., Burnett, S., & Viding, E. (2010). Feeling, caring, knowing: Different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1188–1197. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x
  • Jones, R. L., Scullin, M. H., & Meissner, C. A. (2011). Evidence of differential performance on simultaneous and sequential lineups for individuals with autism-spectrum traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 537–540. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.013
  • Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 110, 197–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006
  • Kim, E., Kyeong, S., Cheon, K. A., Park, B., Oh, M. K., Won Chun, J., … Song, D. H. (2016). Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 621, 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.04.026
  • Knobe, J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis, 63, 190–193. doi:10.1111/1467-8284.00419 doi: 10.1093/analys/63.3.190
  • Knobe, J. (2006). The concept of intentional action: A case study in the uses of folk psychology. Philosophical Studies, 130, 203–231. doi: 10.1007/s11098-004-4510-0
  • Knobe, J. (2010). Person as scientist, person as moralist. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 315–329. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000907
  • Koster-Hale, J., Saxe, R., Dungan, J., & Young, L. (2013). Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 110, 5648–5653. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207992110
  • Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
  • Lanteri, A. (2012). Three-and-a-half folk concepts of intentional action. Philosophical Studies: An International Journal for Philosophy in the Analytic Tradition, 158(1), 17–30. doi: 10.1007/s11098-010-9664-3
  • Leslie, A., Knobe, J., & Cohen, A. (2006). Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect. Theory of mind and moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17, 5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01722.x
  • Lockwood, P. L., Bird, G., Bridge, M., & Viding, E. (2013). Dissecting empathy: High levels of psychopathic and autistic traits are characterized by difficulties in different social information processing domains. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 760. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00760
  • Malle, B., & Knobe, J. (1997). The folk concept of intentionality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 101–121. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1996.1314
  • Malle, B., & Nelson, S. (2003). Judging mens rea: The tension between folk concepts and legal concepts of intentionality. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 21, 563–580. doi: 10.1002/bsl.554
  • Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2016). Mental state understanding and moral judgment in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1478. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01478
  • Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., De Berardis, D., et al. (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 8–791. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00791
  • Mele, A., & Cushman, F. A. (2007). Intentional action, folk judgments and stories: Sorting things out. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 31, 184–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-4975.2007.00147.x
  • Miller, G., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976). Language and perception. Harvard: Harvard University Press.
  • Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 108, 2688–2692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011734108
  • Nadelhoffer, T. (2006). Bad acts, blameworthy agents and intentional actions: Some problems for juror impartiality. Philosophical Explorations, 9, 203–220. doi: 10.1080/13869790600641905
  • Ngo, L., Kelly, M., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Huettel, S. A., Coutlee, C. G., Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2015). Two distinct moral mechanisms for ascribing and denying intentionality. Scientific Reports, 5, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/srep17390
  • Nichols, S., & Ulatowski, J. (2007). Intuitions and individual differences: The knobe effect revisited. Mind & Language, 22, 346–365. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00312.x
  • Patil, I., Calò, M., Fornasier, F., Young, L., & Silani, G. (2017). Neuroanatomical correlates of forgiving unintentional harms. Scientific Reports, 7, 45967. doi: 10.1038/srep45967
  • Patil, I., Melsbach, J., Hennig-Fast, K., & Silani, G. (2016). Divergent roles of autistic and alexithymic traits in utilitarian moral judgments in adults with autism. Scientific Reports, 6, 23637. doi: 10.1038/srep23637
  • Pedreño, C., Pousa, E., Navarro, J. B., Pàmias, M., & Obiols, J. E. (2017). Exploring the components of advanced theory of mind in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 2401–2409. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3156-7
  • Pettit, D., & Knobe, J. (2009). The pervasive impact of moral judgment. Mind & Language, 24, 586–604. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01375.x
  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553
  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a “theory of mind”? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512
  • Ritvo, R. A., Ritvo, E. R., Guthrie, D., Ritvo, M. J., Hufnagel, D. H., McMahon, W., … Eloff, J. (2011). The Ritvo autism asperger diagnostic scale-revised (RAADS-R): A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults: An international validation study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1076–1089. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1133-5
  • Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 709–715. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8
  • Rutter, M. (1978). Language disorder and infantile autism. In M. Rutter & E. Shopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 1–26). New York: Plenum Press.
  • Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayld, M. C., Besche, C., & Widlöcher, D. (1997). Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: A non-verbal exploration with comic strips. Schizophrenia Research, 25, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00025-X
  • Schwenck, C., Mergenthaler, J., Keller, K., Zech, J., Salehi, S., Taurines, R., … Freitag, C. M. (2012). Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: Group-specific profiles and developmental aspects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 651–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02499.x
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex, 46(5), 668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Yaniv, S., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). Empathy deficits in asperger syndrome: A cognitive profile. NeuroCase, 8, 245–252.
  • Singer, T., & Leiberg, S. (2009). Sharing the emotions of others: The neural bases of empathy. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences IV (pp. 971–984). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Smith, A. (2009). The empathy imbalance hypothesis of autism: A theoretical approach to cognitive and emotional empathy in autistic development. The Psychological Record, 59, 3. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol59/iss3/9 doi: 10.1007/BF03395675
  • South, M., Ozonoff, S., & Mcmahon, W. M. (2007). The relationship between executive functioning, central coherence, and repetitive behaviors in the high-functioning autism spectrum. Autism, 11, 437–451. doi: 10.1177/1362361307079606
  • Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–656. doi: 10.1162/089892998562942
  • Turner, R., & Felisberti, F. M. (2017). Measuring mindreading: A review of behavioral approaches to testing cognitive and affective mental state attribution in neurologically typical adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(47), doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00047
  • Vellante, M., Baron-Cohen, S., Melis, M., Marrone, M., Petretto, D. R., Masala, C., & Preti, A. (2013). The reading the mind in the eyes test: Systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18(4), 326–354. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.721728
  • Weisberg, D. S., & Leslie, A. M. (2012). The role of victims’ emotions in preschoolers’ moral judgments. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3, 439–455. doi: 10.1007/s13164-012-0101-8
  • Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 107(15), 6753–6758. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914826107
  • Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 104, 8235–8240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701408104
  • Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2009). Innocent intentions: A correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2065–2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.020
  • Zalla, T., & Leboyer, M. (2011). Judgment of intentionality and moral evaluation in individuals with high functioning autism. Review of Philosophical Psychology, 4, 681–698. doi: 10.1007/s13164-011-0048-1
  • Zalla, T., & Sperduti, M. (2013). The amygdala and the relevance detection theory of autism: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 894. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00894
  • Zalla, T., Stopin, A., Ahade, S., Sav, A. M., & Leboyer, M. (2009). Faux pas detection and intentional action in asperger syndrome. A replication on a French sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 373–382. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0634-y
  • Zucchelli, M. M., Nori, R., Gambetti, E., Marinello, F., & Giusberti, F. (2015). The influence of theory of mind ability and time on intentionality judgments. Paper presented to the XII annual conference of the Italian Association of Cognitive Sciences (AISC) language, cognition & society. Retrieved from https://aisc2015.wordpress.com/

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.