3,388
Views
25
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Explanations for cultural differences in thinking: Easterners’ dialectical thinking and Westerners’ linear thinking

&
Pages 487-506 | Received 12 May 2018, Accepted 26 May 2019, Published online: 04 Jun 2019

References

  • Adair, W. L. (2003). Integrative sequences and negotiation outcome in same-and mixed culture negotiations. International Journal of Conflict Management, 14, 273–296. doi.org/10.1108/eb022902
  • Atran, S., & Medin, D. L. (2008). The native mind and the cultural construction of nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bagozzi, R. P., Wong, N., & Yi, Y. (1999). The role of culture and gender in the relationship between positive and negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 641–672. doi.org/10.1080/026999399379023
  • Berry, J. W. (1994). Ecology of individualism and collectivism. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç Kagitçibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism (pp. 77–84). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  • Boucher, H. C., Peng, K., Shi, J., & Wang, L. (2009). Culture and implicit self-Esteem. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 24–45. doi: 10.1177/0022022108326195
  • Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 11, 264–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00266.x
  • Choi, I., Dalal, R., Kim-Prieto, C., & Park, H. (2003). Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 46–59. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.46
  • Choi, I., Koo, M., & Choi, J. A. (2007). Individual differences in analytic versus holistic thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 691–705. doi: 10.1177/0146167206298568
  • Choi, I., & Nisbett, E. R. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the corresponding bias and actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 949–960. doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514/79.6.890 doi: 10.1177/0146167298249003
  • Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories and recognition of contradiction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 890–905. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.890
  • Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 47–63. doi:0033-2909/99/S3.00 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47
  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.
  • Church, A. T., Alvarez, J. M., Katigbak, M. S., Mastor, K. A., Cabrera, H. F., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., … Buchanan, A. L. (2012). Self-concept consistency and short-term stability in eight cultures. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 556–570. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.06.003
  • Crook, S., Spencer-Rodgers, J., & Peng, K. (2014). Dialectical thinking in Arabic-, English-, and Chinese-speaking nations. Approaches: Revue des Sciences Humaines, 14, 12–27.
  • Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
  • English, T., & Chen, S. (2007). Culture and self-concept stability: Consistency across and within contexts among Asian Americans and European Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 478–490. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.478
  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
  • Friedman, M., Chen, H.-C., & Vaid, J. (2006). Proverb preferences across cultures: Dialecticality or poeticality? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 353–359. doi: 10.3758/BF03193856
  • Gallois, C., Ogay, T., & Giles, H. (2005). Communication accommodation theory: A look back and a look ahead. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Theorizing about intercultural communication (pp. 121–148). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publication.
  • Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., & Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1225–1244. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1225
  • Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., … Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 1100–1104. doi: 10.1126/science.1197754
  • Gudykunst, W. B. (1991). Bridging differences: Effective intergroup communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
  • Gudykunst, W. B., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Culture and interpersonal communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  • Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. Garden City, NJ: Anchor Books/Doubleday.
  • Hall, E. T. (2000). Context and meaning. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (9th ed., pp. 34–43). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing.
  • Hall, E. T., & Hall, M. R. (1990). Understanding cultural differences. Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press.
  • Harari, Y. N. (2015). Sapiens: A brief history of humankind. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.
  • Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
  • Hong, Y.-Y., Benet-Martinez, V., Chiu, C.-Y., & Morris, M. (2003). Boundaries of cultural influence. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 453–464. doi: 10.1177/0022022103034004005
  • Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709
  • Ikegami, Y. (2000). Invitation to theories of Japanese. Tokyo: Kodansha. (in Japanese).
  • Inoue, T. (2011). How did Han people integrate non-Han people regarded as barbarians into greater China? Historical Journal, 733, 57–70. (in Japanese).
  • Ishii, K., Reyes, J. A., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Spontaneous attention to word content versus emotional tone. Psychological Science, 14, 39–46. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01416
  • Ji, L.-J. (2008). The leopard cannot change his spots, or can he? Culture and the development of lay theories of change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 613–622. doi: 10.1177/0146167207313935
  • Ji, L.-J., Nisbett, R. E., & Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psychological Science, 12, 450–456. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00384
  • Ji, L.-J., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 943–955. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.943
  • Kitayama, S., & Ishii, K. (2002). Word and voices: Spontaneous attention to emotional speech in two cultures. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 29–59. doi: 10.1080/0269993943000121
  • Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K., & Ramaswamy, J. (2006). Voluntary settlement and the spirit of independence: Evidence from Japan’s “Northern Frontier”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 369–384. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.369
  • Kittler, M. G., Rygl, D., & Mackinnon, A. (2011). Special review article: Beyond culture or beyond control? Reviewing the use of Hall’s high-/low-context concept. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 11, 63–82. doi: 10.1177/1470595811398797
  • Knight, N., & Nisbett, R. E. (2007). Culture, class and cognition: Evidence from Italy. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 283–291. doi: 10.1163/156853707X208512
  • Korac-Kakabadse, N., Kouzmin, A., Korac- Kakabadse, A., & Savery, L. (2001). Low- and high-context communication patterns: Towards mapping cross-cultural encounters. Cross Cultural Management: An International Journal, 8, 3–24. doi: 10.1108/13527600110797218
  • Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., & Schubert, B. (2001). The semantic-procedural interface model of the self: The role of self-knowledge for context-dependent versus context-independent modes of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 397–409. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.397
  • Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self-influences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 492–499. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00011-2
  • Langer, E. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Leung, A. K. Y., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and creativity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 723–741. doi: 10.1177/0022022110361707
  • Leung, A. K. Y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169–181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.3.169
  • Ma-Kellams, C., Spencer-Rodgers, J., & Peng, K. (2011). I am against us?: Unpacking cultural differences in ingroup favoritism via dialecticism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 15–27. doi: 10.1177/0146167210388193
  • Manktelow, K. I., & Over, D. E. (1991). Social roles and utilities in reasoning with deontic conditionals. Cognition, 39, 85–105. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90039-7
  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
  • Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922–934. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922
  • Mercier, H., Yama, H., Kawasaki, Y., Adachi, K., & Van der Henst, J.-B. (2012). Is the use of averaging in advice taking modulated by culture? Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12, 1–16. doi: 10.1163/156853712X633893
  • Mercier, H., Zhang, J., Qu, Y., Lu, P., & Van der Henst, J.-B. (2015). Do Easterners and Westerners treat contradiction differently? Journal of Cognition and Culture, 15, 45–63. doi:10.1163/156853712X633893 doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342140
  • Miyamoto, Y. (2013). Culture and analytic versus holistic cognition: Toward multilevel analysis of cultural influences. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 131–188.
  • Miyamoto, Y., Knoepfler, C. A., Ishii, K., & Ji, L.-J. (2013). Cultural variation in the focus on goals versus processes of actions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 707–719. doi: 10.1177/0146167213483579
  • Miyamoto, Y., Uchida, Y., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2010). Culture and mixed emotions: Co-occurrence of positive and negative emotions in Japan and the United States. Emotion, 10, 404–415. doi: 10.1037/a0018430
  • Molinsky, A. (2007). Cross-cultural code switching: The psychological challenges of adapting behavior in foreign cultural interactions. Academy of Management Review, 32, 622–640. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.24351878
  • Morris, W. M., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949
  • Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., & Nisbett, R. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible to individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 6192–6197. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001911107
  • Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. New York: The Free Press.
  • Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
  • Norenzayan, A., & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 763–784. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763
  • Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26, 653–684. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2605_4
  • Oishi, S., & Graham, J. (2010). Social ecology: Lost and found in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 356–377. doi: 10.1177/1745691610374588
  • Oyserman, D. (2011). Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. European Review of Social Psychology, 22, 164–214. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2011.627187
  • Paletz, S. B. F., Bogue, K., Miron-Spektor, E., & Spencer-Rodgers, J. (2018). Dialectical thinking and creativity from many perspectives: Contradiction and tension. In J. Spencer-Rodgers, & K. Peng (Eds.), The psychological and cultural foundations of East Asian cognition (pp. 267–308). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Paul, K., Meyskens, M., & Robbins, S. (2011). Components of a global mindset: Corporate social responsibility and cross-cultural sensitivity. Journal of International Business and Cultural Studies, 5, 1–18.
  • Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741
  • Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  • Putterman, L., & Weil, D. N. (2010). Post-1500 population flows and the long run determinants of economic growth and inequality. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125, 1627–1682. doi: 10.1162/qjec.2010.125.4.1627
  • Rösch, M., & Segler, K. G. (1987). Communication with Japanese. Management International Review, 27, 56–67.
  • Rosenbloom, B., & Larsen, T. (2003). Communication in international business-to-business marketing channels Industrial Marketing Management, 32, 309–315. doi: 10.1016/S0019-8501(01)00202-4
  • Rychlowska, M., Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, D., Hess, U., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Kamble, S., … Niedenthal, P. M. (2015). Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, E2429–E2436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1413661112
  • Schug, J., Yuki, M., & Maddux, W. (2010). Relational mobility explains between- and within-culture differences in self-disclosure to close friends. Psychological Science, 21, 1471–1478. doi: 10.1177/0956797610382786
  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., Boucher, H. C., Mori, S. C., Wang, L., & Peng, K. (2009). The dialectical self-concept: Contradiction, change, and holism in East Asian cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 29–44. doi: 10.1177/0146167208325772
  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., & Wang, L. (2010). Naïve dialecticism and the co-occurrences of positive and negative emotions across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 109–115. doi: 10.1177/0022022109349508
  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and East-West differences in psychological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1416–1432. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264243
  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., Williams, M., & Peng, K. (2010). Cultural differences in expectations of change and tolerance for contradiction: A decade of empirical research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 296–312. doi: 10.1177/1088868310362982
  • Suzuki, S., Takemura, K., & Hamamura, T. (2017). Cultural differences in dialecticism. Unpublished data.
  • Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344, 603–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850
  • Ting-Toomey, S. (1999). Communicating across cultures. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649–655. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649
  • Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individual and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
  • Uskul, A. K., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 8552–8556. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803874105
  • Varnum, M. E. W., Grossman, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 9–13. doi: 10.1177/0963721409359301
  • Wong, W.-C. (2006). Understanding dialectical thinking from a cultural-historical perspective. Philosophical Psychology, 19, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/09515080500462420
  • Würtz, E. (2006). Intercultural communication on web sites: A cross-cultural analysis of web sites from high-context cultures and low-context cultures. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11, 274–299. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00013.x doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.tb00313.x
  • Yama, H. (2018). Thinking and reasoning across cultures. In L. J. Ball, & V. A. Thompson (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 624–638). Oxon: Routledge.
  • Yama, H., Manktelow, K. I., Mercier, H., Van der Henst, J.-B., Do, K. S., Kawasaki, Y., & Adachi, K. (2010). A cross-cultural study of hindsight bias and conditional probabilistic reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 16, 346–371. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2010.526786
  • Zakaria, N. (2017). Emergent patterns of switching behaviors and intercultural communication style of global virtual teams during distributed decision making. Journal of International Management, 23, 350–366. doi: 10.1016/j.intman.2016.09.002
  • Zakaria, N., & Cogburn, D. L. (2010). Context-dependent vs. content-dependent: An exploration of the cultural behavioural patterns of online intercultural communication using E-mail. International Journal of Business and Systems Research, 4, 330–347. doi: 10.1504/IJBSR.2010.032954
  • Zakaria, N., & Yusof, S. A. M. (2014). Can we count on you at a distance? The impact of culture on formation of swift trust within global virtual teams. In R. L. Griffith, & J. L. Wildman (Eds.), Leading global teams: Translating the multidisciplinary science to practice (pp. 253–268). New York: Springer.
  • Zakaria, N., & Yusof, S. A. M. (in press). Crossing cultural boundaries using the internet: Toward building a model of swift trust formation in global virtual teams. Journal of International Management. doi.org/10.1016/j.intman.2018.10.004
  • Zell, E., Su, R., Li, H., Ho, M.-H. R., Hong, S., Kumkale, T., … Albarracín, D. (2013). Cultural differences in attitudes toward action and inaction. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 521–528. doi: 10.1177/1948550612468774
  • Zhang, B., Galbraith, N., Yama, H., Wang, L., & Manktelow, K. I. (2015). Dialectical thinking: A cross-cultural study of Japanese, Chinese, and British students. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27, 771–779. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1025792

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.