294
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Dynamics of priming-effect for subliminally presented ambiguous pictures

&
Pages 199-213 | Received 12 Apr 2019, Accepted 18 Dec 2019, Published online: 29 Jan 2020

References

  • Allakhverdov, V. M. (2000). Soznanie kak paradoks [consciousness as a paradox]. St. Petersburg: DNK Press.
  • Allakhverdov, V. M. (2008). Awareness as a result of choice. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 1(1), 136–152. doi:10.11621/pir.2008.0009
  • Allakhverdov, V. M., Filippova, M. G., Gershkovich, V. A., Karpinskaya, V. Y., Scott, T. V., & Vladykina, N. P. (2019). Consciousness, learning, and control: On the path to a theory. In A. Cleermans, V. Allakhverdov, & M. Kuvaldina (Eds.), Implicit learning: 50 years on (pp. 71–108). NJ: Routledge.
  • Atas, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 289–305. doi: 10.1037/a0038654.supp
  • Barenholtz, E., & Islam, M. (2014). Parallel processing of multiple object identities from an ambiguous image: Evidence from negative priming in a lexical decision task. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 1362–1362. doi:10.1167/14.10.1362
  • Brocher, A., & Koenig, J. P. (2016). Word meaning frequencies affect negative compatibility effects In masked priming. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 50–66. doi: 10.5709/acp-0186-x
  • Cacciamani, L., Mojica, A. J., Sanguinetti, J. L., & Peterson, M. A. (2014). Semantic access occurs outside of awareness for the ground side of a figure. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(8), 2531–2547. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0743-y
  • Chao, H.-F. (2013). Locus of single-prime negative priming: The role of perceptual form. Acta Psychologica, 143(3), 303–309. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.010
  • D'Angelo, M. C., & Milliken, B. (2012). Context-specific control in the single-prime negative-priming procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 887–910. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.630478
  • Eckstein, D., Kubat, M., & Perrig, W. J. (2011). Visible homonyms are ambiguous, subliminal homonyms are not: A close look at priming. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1327–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.05.010
  • Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101(2), 293–313. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00009-8
  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1737–1747. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.6.1737
  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(3), 514–520. doi: 10.3758/BF03196307
  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology, 64(1), 7–26. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00100-5
  • Filippova, M. G. (2011). Does unconscious information affect cognitive activity?: A study using experimental priming. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14(1), 20–36. doi: 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n1.2
  • Filippova, M. G., Kostina, D. I., & Mezentseva, M. P. (in press). Dinamika uznavaniya nezamechennykh znacheniy dvoystvennykh izobrazheniy. [The recognition dynamics for unnoticed meanings of ambiguous figures]. Psikhologicheskiy zhurnal.
  • Filippova, M. G., & Moroshkina, NВ. (2015). Osoznavajemaya i neosoznavajemaya mnogoznachnost: dva vida kodnitivnogo kontrolya. [Identified and unidentified ambiguity: Two types of cognitive control]. Sibirski psihologicheskiy jurnal, 56, 37–55. doi:10.17223/17267080/56/4
  • Frings, C., & Eder, A. B. (2009). The time-course of masked negative priming. Experimental Psychology, 56(5), 301–306. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.5.301
  • Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., & Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm: An update and implications for selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(6), 1577–1597. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0841-4
  • Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2005). Negative priming with masked distractor-only prime trials: Awareness moderates negative priming. Experimental Psychology, 52(2), 131–139. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.2.131
  • Grassini, S., Railo, H., Valli, K., Revonsuo, A., & Koivisto, M. (2019). Visual features and perceptual context modulate attention towards evolutionarily relevant threatening stimuli: Electrophysiological evidence. Emotion, 19(2), 348–364. doi: 10.1037/emo0000434
  • Herzog, M. H., Kammer, T., & Scharnowski, F. (2016). Time slices: What is the duration of a percept? PLoS Biology, 14(4), e1002433. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002493
  • Jaśkowski, P., Białuńska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, R. (2008). Mask-and distractor-triggered inhibitory processes in the priming of motor responses: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 70–85. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00595.x
  • Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 255–269. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.131.2.255
  • Koivisto, M., & Grassini, S. (2018). Unconscious response priming during continuous flash suppression. PloS One, 13(2), e0192201. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192201
  • Kostina, D. I., & Allakhverdov, V. M. (2016). Negativnyy prayming-effekt kak proyavleniye posledeystviya negativnogo vybora. [Negative priming effect as a manifestation of the negative choice after effect]. Peterburgskiy psikhologicheskiy zhurnal, (17), 69–103.
  • Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., Bode, S., & Mattler, U. (2013). Neural correlates of control operations in inverse priming with relevant and irrelevant masks. NeuroImage, 64, 197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.018
  • Krüger, D., & Mattler, U. (2012). Inverse cue priming is not limited to masks with relevant features. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1207–1221. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.003
  • Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(3), 545–557. doi: 10.3758/BF03193330
  • Lowe, D. G. (1985). Further investigations of inhibitory mechanisms in attention. Memory and Cognition, 13(1), 74–80. doi: 10.3758/BF03198446
  • Marcel, A. J. (1980). Explaining selective effects of prior context on perception: The need to distinguish conscious and pre-conscious processes. In J. Requin (Ed.), Anticipation and behavior (pp. 411–432). Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.
  • May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118(1), 35–54. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.35
  • Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105(2), 203–229. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.203
  • Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 565–576. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.565
  • Ortells, J. J., Abad, M. J., Noguera, C., & Lupiáñez, J. (2001). Influence of prime–probe stimulus onset asynchrony and prime precuing manipulations on semantic priming effects with words in a lexical-decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 75–91. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.75
  • Ortells, J. J., Fox, E., Noguera, C., & Abad, M. J. (2003). Repetition priming effects from attended vs. ignored single words in a semantic categorization task. Acta Psychologica, 114(2), 185–210. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.08.002
  • Peremen, Z., & Lamy, D. (2014). Comparing unconscious processing during continuous flash suppression and meta-contrast masking just under the limen of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 969. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00969
  • Peterson, M. A., Cacciamani, L., Mojica, A. J., & Sanguinetti, J. L. (2012). Meaning can be accessed for the ground side of a figure. Journal of Gestalt Theory, 34(3), 297–314.
  • Peterson, M. A., & Kim, J. H. (2001). On what is bound in figures and grounds. Visual Cognition, 8(3-5), 329–348. doi: 10.1080/13506280143000034
  • Peterson, M. A., & Skow, E. (2008). Inhibitory competition between shape properties in figure-ground perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 251–267. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.251
  • Rieger, J. W., Braun, C., Bülthoff, H. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2005). The dynamics of visual pattern masking in natural scene processing: A magnetoencephalography study. Journal of Vision, 5(3), 275–286. doi: 10.1167/5.3.10
  • Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2000). A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 62(7), 1367–1382. doi: 10.3758/BF03212139
  • Shimojo, S. (2014). Postdiction: Its implications on visual awareness, hindsight, and sense of agency. Frontiers in Psychology, 196, 1–19. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00196
  • Simpson, G. B., & Burgess, C. (1985). Activation and selection processes in the recognition of ambiguous words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 28–39. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.11.1.28
  • Sumner, P., & Brandwood, T. (2008). Oscillations in motor priming: Positive rebound follows the inhibitory phase in the masked prime paradigm. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40(6), 484–490. doi: 10.3200/JMBR.40.6.484-490
  • Tal, A., & Bar, M. (2014). The proactive brain and the fate of dead hypotheses. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 8, 138. doi:10.3389/fncom.2014.00138
  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(4), 571–590. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400920
  • Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54(2), 321–343. doi: 10.1080/713755969
  • Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1987). Ambiguous words in context: An event-related potential analysis of the time course of meaning activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(2), 188–208. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90123-9
  • Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(10), 6275–6280. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0931489100
  • Wernicke, M., & Mattler, U. (2019). Masking procedures can influence priming effects besides their effects on conscious perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 71, 92–108. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.03.009
  • Yee, P. L. (1991). Semantic inhibition of ignored words during a figure classification task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43(1), 127–153. doi: 10.1080/14640749108401002

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.