91
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

School-age children can form time-based event expectancy for context-atypical foreperiods

, &
Pages 24-37 | Received 23 Feb 2020, Accepted 19 Nov 2020, Published online: 07 Dec 2020

References

  • Aufschnaiter, S., Kiesel, A., Dreisbach, G., Wenke, D., & Thomaschke, R. (2018). Time-based expectancy in temporally structured task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(6), 856–870. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000494
  • Aufschnaiter, S., Kiesel, A., & Thomaschke, R. (2018). Transfer of time-based task expectancy across different timing environments. Psychological Research, 82(1), 230–243. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0895-1
  • Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 296–312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x
  • Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte Carlo investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 566–579. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.566
  • Creelman, C. D. (1962). Human discrimination of auditory duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34(5), 582–593. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1918172
  • Donohue, S. E., Woldorff, M. G., & Mitroff, S. R. (2010). Video game players show more precise multisensory temporal processing abilities. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(4), 1120–1129. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.4.1120
  • Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception in children: A neurodevelopmental approach. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 220–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.023
  • Droit-Volet, S., Jennifer, T., & Coull, J. T. (2016). Distinct developmental trajectories for explicit and implicit timing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.010
  • Droit-Volet, S., & Zélanti, P. S. (2013). Development of time sensitivity and information processing speed. PLoS One, 8(8), e71424. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071424
  • Dye, M. W., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Differential development of visual attention skills in school- age children. Vision Research, 50(4), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.010
  • Gautier, T., & Droit-Volet, S. (2002). Attention and time estimation in 5- and 8-year-old children: A dual-task procedure. Behavioural Processes, 58(1–2), 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00002-5
  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84(3), 279–325. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.279
  • Johnson, K. A., Bryan, M., Polonowita, K., Decroupet, D., & Coull, J. T. (2016). Isochronous sequential presentation helps children orient their attention in time. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1417. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01417.
  • Johnson, K. A., Burrowes, E., & Coull, J. T. (2015). Children can implicitly, but not voluntarily, direct attention in time. PLoS One, 10(4), e0123625. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123625
  • Kunchulia, M., Parkosadze, K., & Thomaschke, R. (2019). The time-based event expectancies decline with aging. Timing & Time Perception, 7(1), 71–85. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181123
  • Kunchulia, M., Tatishvili, T., Lomidze, N., Parkosadze, K., & Thomaschke, R. (2017). Time-based event expectancies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 235(9), 2877–2882. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5024-2
  • Kunchulia, M., Tatishvili, T., Parkosadze, K., Lomidze, N., & Thomaschke, R. (2020). Children with autism spectrum disorder show increased sensitivity to time-based predictability. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 66, 204–213. https://doi.org/10.1080/20473869.2018.1564447
  • Kunchulia, M., & Thomaschke, R. (2016). Effects of alcohol intake on time-based event expectations. Experimental Brain Research, 234(4), 937–944. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4522-3
  • Langner, R., Steinborn, M. B., Chatterjee, A., Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2010). Mental fatigue and temporal preparation in simple reaction-time performance. Acta Psychologica, 133(1), 64–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.10.001
  • Los, S. A., Knol, D. L., & Boers, R. M. (2001). The foreperiod effect revisited: Conditioning as a basis for nonspecific preparation. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 121–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00029-9
  • Los, S. A., & van den Heuvel, C. E. (2001). Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 370–386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.370
  • Mento, G., Astle, D. E., & Scerif, G. (2018). Cross-frequency phase-amplitude coupling as a mechanism for temporal orienting of attention in childhood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(4), 594–602. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01223
  • Mento, G., & Tarantino, V. (2015). Developmental trajectories of internally and externally driven temporal prediction. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0135098. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135098
  • Mento, G., & Vallesib, A. (2016). Spatiotemporally dissociable neural signatures for generating and updating expectation over time in children: A High Density-ERP study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 98–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.02.008
  • Plude, D., Enns, J. T., & Brodeur, D. (1994). The development of selective attention: A life-span overview. Acta Psychologica, 86(2-3), 227–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)90004-3
  • Rattat, A. C., & Tartas, V. (2017). Temporal categorization of familiar actions by children and adults. Timing & Time Perception, 5(1), 61–76. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002080
  • Roberts, F., & Francis, A. L. (2013). Identifying a temporal threshold of tolerance for silent gaps after requests. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133(6), EL471–EL477. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4802900
  • Roberts, F., Margutti, P., & Takano, S. (2011). Judgments concerning the valence of inter-turn silence across speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese. Discourse Processes, 48(5), 331–354. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002
  • Roberts, F., & Norris, A. (2016). Gendered expectations for "agreeableness" in response to requests and opinions. Communication Research Reports, 33, 16–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824096.2015.1117437
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Psychology Software Tools Inc.
  • Seow, S. C. (2008). Designing and engineering time: The psychology of time perception in software. Addison-Wesley Professional.
  • Shahar, N., Meyer, J., Hildebrandt, M., & Rafaely, V. (2012). Detecting system failures from durations and binary cues. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 70(8), 552–556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2012.03.001
  • Steinborn, M. B., & Langner, R. (2011). Distraction by irrelevant sound during foreperiods selectively impairs temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 405–418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.008
  • Steinborn, M. B., & Langner, R. (2012). Arousal modulates temporal preparation under increased time uncertainty: Evidence from higher-order sequential foreperiod effects. Acta Psychologica, 139(1), 65–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.10.010
  • Steinborn, M. B., Langner, R., Flehmig, H. C., & Huestegge, L. (2017). Methodology of performance scoring in the d2 Sustained-attention test: Cumulative-reliability functions and practical guidelines. Psychological Assessment. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000482
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129(2), 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.08.005
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 40–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.002
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 94–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.011
  • Szameitat, A. J., Rummel, J., Szameitat, D. P., & Sterr, A. (2009). Behavioral and emotional consequences of brief delays in human-computer interaction. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 67(7), 561–570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.02.004
  • Thomaschke, R., & Dreisbach, G. (2015). The time-event correlation effect is due to temporal expectancy, not to partial transition costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 196–218. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038328
  • Thomaschke, R., & Haering, C. (2014). Predictivity of system delays shortens human response time. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 72(3), 358–365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2013.12.004
  • Thomaschke, R., Kunchulia, M., & Dreisbach, G. (2015). Time-based event expectations employ relative, not absolute, representations of time. Psychonomic Bulletin, & Review, 22(3), 890–895. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0710-6
  • Thomaschke, R., Wagener, A., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). The scope and precision of specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(3), 953–964. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-010-0079-1
  • Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs, 77(13), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093864
  • Trick, L. M., & Enns, J. (1998). Lifespan changes in attention: The visual search task. Cognitive Development, 13(3), 369–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90016-8
  • Vallesi, A., & Shallice, T. (2007). Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: Deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1377
  • Vesper, C., Abramova, E., Bütepage, J., Ciardo, F., Crossey, B., Effenberg, A., Hristova, D., Karlinsky, A., Mcellin, L., Nijssen, S. R., Schmitz, L., & Wahn, B. (2017). Joint action: Mental representations, shared information and general mechanisms for coordinating with others. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02039

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.