268
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Visual salience accelerates lexical processing and subsequent integration: an eye-movement study

ORCID Icon, , , &
Pages 146-156 | Received 14 May 2020, Accepted 18 Jan 2021, Published online: 27 Jan 2021

References

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Cashen, V. M., & Leicht, K. L. (1970). Role of the isolation effect in a formal educational setting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61(6 PART 1), 484–486. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030286
  • Cavicchio, F., Melcher, D., & Poesio, M. (2014). The effect of linguistic and visual salience in visual world studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(176), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00176
  • Chen, L., & Yang, Y. (2015). Emphasizing the only character: Emphasis, attention and contrast. Cognition, 136, 222–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.015
  • Filik, R., Purdy, K., Gale, A., & Gerrett, D. (2006). Labeling of medicines and patient safety: Evaluating methods of reducing drug name confusion. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 48(1), 39–47. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872006776412199
  • Fitzsimmons, G., Weal, M. J., & Drieghe, D. (2019). The impact of hyperlinks on reading text. PLoS ONE, 14(2), e0210900. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210900
  • Fraundorf, S. H., Benjamin, A. S., & Watson, D. G. (2013). What happened (and what did not): Discourse constraints on encoding of plausible alternatives. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(3), 196–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.003
  • Gaddy, M. L., van den Broek, P., & Sung, Y.-C. (2001). The influence of text cues on the allocation of attention during. In T. Sanders, J. Schilperoord, & W. Spooren (Eds.), Text representation: Linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects (pp. 89–110). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Golding, J. M., & Fowler, S. B. (1992). The limited facilitative effect of typographical signals. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17(2), 99–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-476X(92)90052-Z
  • Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2014). Semantic preview benefit during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), 166–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033670
  • Inhoff, A. W. (1984). Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(5), 612–624. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90382-7
  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discoures comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163
  • Lemarié, J., Lorch, R. F. J., Eyrolle, H. H., & Virbel, J. (2008). SARA: A text-based and reader-based theory of signaling. Educational Psychologist, 43(1), 27–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520701756321
  • Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2020). An integrated model of word processing and eye-movement control during Chinese reading. Psychological Review, 127(6), 1139–1162. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000248
  • Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58(4), 525–552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.003
  • Lima, S. D., & Inhoff, A. W. (1985). Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word-initial letter sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(3), 272–285. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.3.272
  • Liversedge, S. P., Paterson, K. B., & Pickering, M. J. (1998). Eye movements and measures of reading time. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 55–100). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-008043361-5/50004-3
  • Lorch, R. F. (1989). Text-signaling devices and their effects on reading and memory processes. Educational Psychology Review, 1(3), 209–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01320135
  • Lorch, R. F., Pugzles, E., & Klusewitz, M. A. (1995). Effects of typographical cues on reading and recall of text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(1), 51–64. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1995.1003
  • Macaya, M., & Perea, M. (2014). Does bold emphasis facilitate the process of visual-word recognition? Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17(e2), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2014.2
  • Mackay, M., Cerf, M., & Koch, C. (2012). Evidence for two distinct mechanisms directing gaze in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 12(4), 9–9. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.4.9
  • McAteer, E. (1989). Typeface effects in written language: Functions of typface change for signalling meaning within text [University of Glasgow]. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/id/eprint/1105
  • McAteer, E. (1992). Typeface emphasis and information focus in written language. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6(4), 345–359. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350060406
  • Pelli, D. G., Burns, C. W., Farell, B., & Moore-Page, D. C. (2006). Feature detection and letter identification. Vision Research, 46(28), 4646–4674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.04.023
  • Perea, M., & Rosa, E. (2002). Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition? Perception and Psychophysics, 64(5), 785–794. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194745
  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. Oxford University Press.
  • Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430701530730
  • Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157
  • Rawson, K. A., Thomas, R. C., & Jacoby, L. L. (2015). The power of examples: Illustrative examples enhance conceptual learning of declarative concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 483–504. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9273-3
  • Rayner, K., & Schotter, E. R. (2014). Semantic preview benefit in reading English: The effect of initial letter capitalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1617–1628. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036763
  • Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Morris, R. K., Schmauder, A. R., & Clifton, C. (1989). Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(3-4), SI21–SI49. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968908406362
  • Sanford, A. J. S., Sanford, A. J., Molle, J., & Emmott, C. (2006). Shallow processing and attention capture in written and spoken discourse. Discourse Processes, 42(2), 109–130. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp4202_2
  • Schütz, A. C., Trommershäuser, J., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Dynamic integration of information about salience and value for saccadic eye movements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(19), 7547–7552. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115638109
  • Sivak, M. (1996). The information that drivers use: Is it indeed 90% visual? Perception, 25(9), 1081–1089. https://doi.org/10.1068/p251081
  • Tatler, B. W., Hayhoe, M. M., Land, M. F., & Ballard, D. H. (2011). Eye guidance in natural vision: Reinterpreting salience. Journal of Vision, 11(5), 5–5. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.5.5
  • Uddin, L. Q. (2017). What is salience? In L. Q. Uddin (Ed.), Salience network of the human brain (pp. 1–4). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804593-0.00001-1
  • van den Broek, P. (1995). A ‘landscape’ model of reading comprehension: Inferential processes and the construction of a stable memory representation.. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 36(1), 53–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084723
  • van den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C. R., & Thurlow, R. (1996). A “landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Model of understanding text (pp. 165–187). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Wood, D. K., Gallivan, J. P., Chapman, C. S., Milne, J. L., Culham, J. C., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Visual salience dominates early visuomotor competition in reaching behavior. Journal of Vision, 11(10), 16–16. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.10.16
  • Yeari, M., Oudega, M., & van den Broek, P. (2017). The effect of highlighting on processing and memory of central and peripheral text information: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(4), 365–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12072
  • Zarcone, A., van Schijndel, M., Vogels, J., & Demberg, V. (2016). Salience and attention in surprisal-based accounts of language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(844), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00844

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.