388
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Registered Report

Do bilinguals have an advantage in prospective memory?

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 255-270 | Received 03 Jan 2021, Accepted 12 Dec 2022, Published online: 03 Jan 2023

References

  • Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003
  • Altgassen, M., Phillips, L., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., & Kliegel, M. (2010a). Emotional target cues eliminate age differences in prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 1057–1064. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470211003770920
  • Altgassen, M., Schmitz-Hübsch, M., & Kliegel, M. (2010b). Event-based prospective memory performance in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2(1), 2–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11689-009-9030-y
  • Altgassen, M., Williams, T. I., Bölte, S., & Kliegel, M. (2009). Time-based prospective memory in children with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Impairment, 10(1), 52–58. https://doi.org/10.1375/brim.10.1.52
  • Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
  • Ball, B. H., & Bugg, J. M. (2018). Context cue focality influences strategic prospective memory monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1405–1415. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1442-9
  • Ball, B. H., Li, Y. P., & Bugg, J. M. (2020). Aging and strategic prospective memory monitoring. Memory & Cognition, 48(3), 370–389. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00976-8
  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6), 561–571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004
  • Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12116
  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099
  • Causey, K. B., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2014). Prospective memory in preschool children: Influences of agency, incentive, and underlying cognitive mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 127, 36–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.020
  • Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196772
  • Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2(3), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000334
  • Declerck, M., Grainger, J., Koch, I., & Philipp, A. M. (2017). Is language control just a form of executive control? Evidence for overlapping processes in language switching and task switching. Journal of Memory and Language, 95, 138–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.03.005
  • Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2018). Mechanisms of memory disruption in depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 41(3), 137–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.12.006
  • Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286–290. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00382.x
  • Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Prospective memory and what costs do not reveal about retrieval processes: A commentary on Smith, Hunt, McVay, and McConnell (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1082–1088. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019184
  • Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R., Mayfield, S., Shank, H., Morrisette, N., & Breneiser, J. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 327–342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.327
  • Epir, S., & İskit, Ü. (1972). Wechsler yetişkinler zeka ölçeği Türkçe çevirisinin ön analizi ve üniversite danışmanlık merkezlerindeki uygulama potansiyeli. Hacettepe Sosyal ve Beşeri Bilimler Dergisi, 4(2), 198–205.
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). SAGE Publications.
  • Ford, R. M., Driscoll, T., Shum, D., & Macaulay, C. E. (2012). Executive and theory-of-mind contributions to event-based prospective memory in children: Exploring the self-projection hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 468–489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.10.006
  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133
  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
  • Harrison, T. L., Mullet, H. G., Whiffen, K. N., Ousterhout, H., & Einstein, G. O. (2014). Prospective memory: Effects of divided attention on spontaneous retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 42(2), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0357-y
  • Heathcote, A., Loft, S., & Remington, R. W.. (2015). Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. Psychological Review, 122(2), 376–410. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0038952.
  • Hefer, C., Cohen, A. L., Jaudas, A., & Dreisbach, G. (2017). The flexible engagement of monitoring processes in non-focal and focal prospective memory tasks with salient cues. Acta Psychologica, 179, 42–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.06.008
  • Hicks, J. L., Franks, B. A., & Spitler, S. N. (2017). Prior task experience and comparable stimulus exposure nullify focal and nonfocal prospective memory retrieval differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(10), 1997–2006. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1217891
  • Hisli, N. (1989). The validity and reliability of beck depression inventory for university students. Psikoloji Dergisi, 7(23), 3–13.
  • Hunter Ball, B., & Bugg, J. M. (2018). Context cue focality influences strategic prospective memory monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1405–1415. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1442-9
  • Ihle, A., Ghisletta, P., & Kliegel, M. (2017). Prospective memory and intraindividual variability in ongoing task response times in an adult lifespan sample: The role of cue focality. Memory, 25(3), 370–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1173705
  • Jäger, T., & Kliegel, M. (2008). Time-based and event-based prospective memory across adulthood: Underlying mechanisms and differential costs on the ongoing task. The Journal of General Psychology, 135(1), 4–22. https://doi.org/10.3200/GENP.135.1.4-22
  • Kalia, V., Wilbourn, M. P., & Ghio, K. (2014). Better early or late? Examining the influence of age of exposure and language proficiency on executive function in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(7), 699–713. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.956748
  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
  • Kliegel, M., Altgassen, M., Hering, A., & Rose, N. S. (2011). A process-model based approach to prospective memory impairment in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 49(8), 2166–2177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.024
  • Kliegel, M., & Jäger, T. (2007). The effects of age and cue-action reminders on event-based prospective memory performance in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 22(1), 33–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.08.003
  • Kliegel, M., Mahy, C. E., Voigt, B., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., & Aberle, I. (2013). The development of prospective memory in young schoolchildren: The impact of ongoing task absorption, cue salience, and cue centrality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 792–810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.012
  • Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2002). Complex prospective memory and executive control of working memory: A process model. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 44(2), 303–318.
  • Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. (2015). Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 377–394. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124937
  • Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 651–662. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891100054X
  • Loft, S., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Wait a second: Brief delays in responding reduce focality effects in event-based prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(7), 1432–1447. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.750677
  • López-Rojas, C., Rossi, E., Marful, A., & Bajo, M. T. (2022). Prospective memory in bilinguals and monolinguals: ERP and behavioural correlates of prospective processing in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 225, 105059. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105059
  • Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 588–595. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000010
  • Mackinlay, R. J., Kliegel, M., & Mäntylä, T. (2009). Predictors of time-based prospective memory in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 251–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.08.006
  • Mahy, C. E. V., & Moses, L. J. (2011). Executive functioning and prospective memory in young children. Cognitive Development, 26(3), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.06.002
  • Mahy, C. E. V., Moses, L. J., & Kliegel, M. (2014). The development of prospective memory In children: An executive framework. Developmental Review, 34(4), 305–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.08.001
  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
  • Martin, M., Kliegel, M., & McDaniel, M. A. (2003). The involvement of executive functions in prospective memory performance of adults. International Journal of Psychology, 38(4), 195–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000123
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 14(7), 127–144.
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Sage Publications, Inc.
  • McDaniel, M. A., Shelton, J. T., Breneiser, J. E., Moynan, S., & Balota, D. A. (2011). Focal and nonfocal prospective memory performance in very mild dementia: A signature decline. Neuropsychology, 25(3), 387–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021682
  • McFarland, C. P., & Vasterling, J. J. (2018). Prospective memory in depression: Review of an emerging field†. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 33(7), 912–930. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx118
  • Meier, B., & Rey-Mermet, A. (2012). Beyond monitoring: After-effects of responding to prospective memory targets. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(4), 1644–1653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.09.003
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Mullet, H. G., Scullin, M. K., Hess, T. J., Scullin, R. B., Arnold, K. M., & Einstein, G. O. (2013). Prospective memory and aging: Evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues. Psychology and Aging, 28(4), 910–922. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034347
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
  • Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 8–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
  • Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 313–325. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035224
  • Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2013). The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 622–645. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
  • Rude, S. S., Hertel, P. T., Jarrold, W., Covich, J., & Hedlund, S. (1999). Depression-related impairments in prospective memory. Cognition & Emotion, 13(3), 267–276. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379276
  • Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., Shelton, J. T., & Lee, J. H. (2010). Focal/nonfocal cue effects in prospective memory: Monitoring difficulty or different retrieval processes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And Cognition, 36(3), 736–749. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018971
  • Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 347–361. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347
  • Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(4), 756–777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.756
  • Smith, R. E., Bayen, U. J., & Martin, C. (2010). The cognitive processes underlying event-based prospective memory in school-age children and young adults: A formal model-based study. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 230–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017100
  • Smith, R. E., Horn, S. S., & Bayen, U. J. (2012). Prospective memory in young and older adults: The effects of ongoing-task load. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19(4), 495–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.633161
  • Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (2014). Prospective memory in young and older adults: The effects of task importance and ongoing task load. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 21(4), 411–431. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2013.827150
  • Strömgren, L. S. (1977). The influence of depression on memory. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 56(2), 109–128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1977.tb06670.x
  • Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 123. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123
  • Türkeş, N., Can, H., Kurt, M., & Dikeç, B. E. (2014). İz Sürme Testi’nin 20–49 yaş aralığında Türkiye için norm belirleme çalışması. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 26(3), 189–196. https://doi.org/10.5080/u7739
  • Uttl, B. (2011). Transparent meta-analysis: Does aging spare prospective memory with focal vs. non-focal cues? PLoS ONE, 6(2), e16618. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016618
  • Wang, L., Altgassen, M., Liu, W., Xiong, W., Akgün, C., & Kliegel, M. (2011). Prospective memory across adolescence: The effects of age and cue focality. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 226–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021306
  • Wechsler, D. (1955). WAIS manual. The Psychological Corporation.
  • Zhao, X., Fu, J., Ma, X., & Maes, J. H. R. (2019). Age differences in prospective memory: A further evaluation of the executive framework. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(5), 680–701. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1648268
  • Zuber, S., Mahy, C. E., & Kliegel, M. (2019). How executive functions are associated with event-based and time-based prospective memory during childhood. Cognitive Development, 50, 66–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.03.001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.