299
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The effect of ageing on confrontation naming in healthy older adults: a three-level meta-analysis

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 480-508 | Received 19 Nov 2021, Accepted 21 Feb 2023, Published online: 09 Mar 2023

References

  • References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the metaanalysis
  • Abrams, L., & Farrell, M. T. (2011). Language processing in normal aging. In J. Guendouzi, F. Loncke, & M. J. William (Eds.), The handbook of psycholinguistic and cognitive processes: Perspectives in communication disorders (pp. 49–73). Taylor and Francis, University of Florida. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203848005.ch3
  • Ainiala, T. (2016). Names in society. In C. Hough (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of names and naming (pp. 1–11). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199656431.013.27
  • *Albert, M. S., Heller, H. S., & Milberg, W. (1988). Changes in naming ability with age. Psychology and Aging, 3(2), 173–178. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.3.2.173
  • Ardila, A., Bertolucci, P. H., Braga, L. W., Castro-Caldas, A., Judd, T., Kosmidis, M. H., Matute, E., Nitrini, R., Ostrosky-Solis, F., & Rosselli, M. (2010). Illiteracy: The neuropsychology of cognition without reading. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(8), 689–712. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acq079
  • Ardila, A., Ostrosky, F., & Mendoza, V. (2000). Learning to read is much more than learning to read: A neuropsychologically-based learning to read method. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 789–801. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677068
  • Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Landeau, B., La Joie, R., Mevel, K., Mézenge, F., Perrotin, A., Desgranges, B., Bartrés-Faz, D., Eustache, F., & Chételat, G. (2013). Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage, 83, 450–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053
  • Ashaie, S., & Obler, L. (2014). Effect of age, education, and bilingualism on confrontation naming in older illiterate and low-educated populations. Behavioural Neurology, 2014, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/970520.
  • Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. J. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12(3), 154–174. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p154
  • *Au, R., Joung, P., Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Kass, R., & Albert, M. L. (1995). Naming ability across the adult life span. Aging & Cognition, 2(4), 300–311. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825589508256605
  • Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12(1), 12–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.12
  • Barresi, B. A., Nicholas, M., Tabor Connor, L., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (2000). Semantic degradation and lexical access in age-related naming failures. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7(3), 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1076/1382-5585(200009)7:3;1-Q;FT169
  • Barresi, B. A., Obler, L. K., Au, R., & Albert, M. L. (1999). Language-related factors influencing naming in adulthood. In H. E. Hamilton (Ed.), Language and communication in Old Age (pp. 77–90). Routledge.
  • Barulli, D., & Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(10), 502–509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012
  • Béland, R., & Lecours, A. R. (1990). The MT-86 β aphasia battery: A subset of normative data in relation to age and level of school education. Aphasiology, 4(5), 439–462. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039008248786
  • Berggren, R., Nilsson, J., & Lövdén, M. (2018). Education does not affect cognitive decline in aging: A Bayesian assessment of the association between education and change in cognitive performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. Article 1138. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01138
  • Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. (2007). Bilingualism and naming: Implications for cognitive assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 209–211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617707070403
  • Binder, J. R. (2017). Current controversies on Wernicke’s area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17(8), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8
  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2021). Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Bornstein, R. A., & Suga, L. J. (1988). Educational level and neuropsychological performance in healthy elderly subjects. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565648809540386
  • *Borod, J. C., Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1980). Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe battery, and the Boston naming test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2(3), 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
  • *Bruffaerts, R., Tyler, L. K., Shafto, M., Tsvetanov, K. A., & Clarke, A. (2019). Perceptual and conceptual processing of visual objects across the adult lifespan. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-50254-5
  • Brydges, C. R. (2019). Effect size guidelines, sample size calculations, and statistical power in gerontology. Innovation in Aging, 3(4), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igz036.
  • Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2004). Aging and language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(1), 21–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301006.x
  • Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2008). Language and aging. In F. I. M. Craik, & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373–443). Psychology Press.
  • *Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 542–579. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90026-G
  • Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475
  • Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., Lindenberger, U., Nyberg, L., Park, D. C., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Rugg, M. D., Steffener, J., &Rajah, M. N. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(11), 701–710. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2
  • Capitani, E., Barbarotto, R., & Laiacona, M. (1996). Does education influence the age-related cognitive decline? A further inquiry. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12(2), 231–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649609540648
  • Card, N. A. (2015). Applied meta-analysis for social science research. Guilford Publications.
  • Carson, N., Leach, L., & Murphy, K. J. (2018). A re-examination of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33(2), 379–388. http://doi.org/10.1002/gps.v33.2
  • Charmaz, K. (2006). The power of names. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 35(4), 396–399. https://doi.org/10.1177/F0891241606286983
  • Cheung, M. W. L. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: A structural equation modeling approach. Psychological Methods, 19(2), 211–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032968
  • Ciesielska, N., Sokołowski, R., Mazur, E., Podhorecka, M., Polak-Szabela, A., & Kędziora-Kornatowska, K. (2016). Is the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) test better suited than the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) detection among people aged over 60? Meta-analysis. Psychiatria Polska, 50(5), 1039–1052. http://doi.org/10.12740/PP/45368
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power for the behavioural sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Cohn, L. D., & Becker, B. J. (2003). How meta-analysis increases statistical power. Psychological Methods, 8(3), 243–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.243
  • Condret-Santi, V., Barbeau, E. J., Matharan, F., Le Goff, M., Dartigues, J. F., & Amieva, H. (2013). Prevalence of word retrieval complaint and prediction of dementia in a population-based study of elderly subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 35(5-6), 313–324. https://doi.org/10.1159/000342594
  • *Constantinidou, F., Christodoulou, M., & Prokopiou, J. (2012). The effects of age and education on executive functioning and oral naming performance in Greek Cypriot adults: The neurocognitive study for the aging. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 64(4), 187–198. https://doi.org/10.1159/000340015
  • Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. Russell Sage Foundation.
  • Corey-Bloom, J., Wiederholt, W. C., Edelstein, S., Salmon, D. P., Cahn, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (1996). Cognitive and functional status of the oldest old. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 44(6), 671–674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb01830.x
  • Cozby, P. C., & Bates, S. (2015). Methods in behavioral research. McGraw-Hill Education, New York.
  • *Cruice, M. N., Worrall, L. E., & Hickson, L. M. H. (2000). Boston naming test results for healthy older Australians: A longitudinal and cross-sectional study. Aphasiology, 14(2), 143–155. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870300401522
  • De Jager, C., Blackwell, A. D., Budge, M. M., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005). Predicting cognitive decline in healthy older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(8), 735–740. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.8.735
  • Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x
  • Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Bmj, 315(7109), 629–634. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x
  • *Farmer, A. (1990). Performance of normal males on the Boston naming test and the word test. Aphasiology, 4(3), 293–296. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039008249081
  • *Fastenau, P. S. (1998). Validity of regression-based norms: An empirical test of the comprehensive norms with older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(6), 906–916. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.20.6.906.1104
  • *Ferré, P., Jarret, J., Brambati, S., Bellec, P., & Joanette, Y. (2020). Functional connectivity of successful picture-naming: Age-specific organization and the effect of engaging in stimulating activities. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/2Ffnagi.2020.535770
  • Feyereisen, P. (1997). A Meta-Analytic Procedure Shows an Age-Related Decline in Picture Naming. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1328–1333. http://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4006.1328
  • Flicker, C., Ferris, S. H., Crook, T., & Bartus, R. T. (1987). Implications of memory and language dysfunction in the naming deficit of senile dementia. Brain and Language, 31(2), 187–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(87)90069-1
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. http://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., McHugh, P. R., & Fanjiang, G. (2001). Mini-mental State Examination User’s Guide. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42(1-3), 61–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-O
  • Gollan, T. H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R. I., & Jernigan, T. L. (2007). The bilingual effect on Boston naming test performance. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 197–208. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617707070038.
  • Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001
  • *Gonzalez-Burgos, L., Hernández-Cabrera, J. A., Westman, E., Barroso, J., & Ferreira, D. (2019). Cognitive compensatory mechanisms in normal aging: A study on verbal fluency and the contribution of other cognitive functions. Aging (Albany NY), 11(12), 4090–4106. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.102040.
  • Goodglass, H. (1980). Naming disorders in aphasia and aging. In L. K. Obler, & M. L. Albert (Eds.), Language and communication in the elderly (pp. 37–45). Lexington Books.
  • Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
  • Goral, M., Libben, G., Obler, L. K., Jarema, G., & Ohayon, K. (2008). Lexical attrition in younger and older bilingual adults. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22(7), 509–522. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200801912237
  • Goral, M., Spiro III, A., Albert, M. L., Obler, L. K., & Connor, L. T. (2007). Change in lexical retrieval skills in adulthood. The Mental Lexicon, 2(2), 215–238. https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.2.2.05gor
  • *Gordon, J. K., & Kindred, N. K. (2011). Word retrieval in ageing: An exploration of the task constraint hypothesis. Aphasiology, 25(6-7), 774–788. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2010.539699
  • *Gordon, J. K., Young, M., & Garcia, C. (2018). Why do older adults have difficulty with semantic fluency? Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 25(6), 803–828. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1374328
  • Goulet, P., Ska, B., & Kahn, H. J. (1994). Is there a decline in picture naming with advancing age?. . Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 37(3), 629–644. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4006.1328
  • Gutherie, A. H., Seely, P. W., Beacham, L. A., Schuchard, R. A., De l’Aune, W. A., & Bacon Moore, A. (2010). Age effects on visual-perceptual processing and confrontation naming. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17(2), 160–190. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580903042643
  • Gutherie, A. H., Seely, P. W., Beacham, L. A., Schuchard, R. A., De l'Aune, W. A., & Bacon Moore, A. (2010). Age Effects on Visual-Perceptual Processing and Confrontation Naming. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17(2), 160–190. http://doi.org/10.1080/13825580903042643
  • Haneuse, S., & Bartell, S. (2011). Designs for the combination of group-and individual-level data. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 22(3), 382–389. https://doi.org/10.1097/ede.0b013e3182125cff
  • Hartling, L., Milne, A., Hamm, M. P., Vandermeer, B., Ansari, M., Tsertsvadze, A., & Dryden, D. M. (2013). Testing the Newcastle Ottawa Scale showed low reliability between individual reviewers. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 66(9), 982–993. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.03.003
  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). Academic Press.
  • Heaton, R. K., Avitable, N., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1999). Further cross-validation of regression-based neuropsychological norms with an update for the Boston Naming Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(4), 571–582. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.21.4.572.882
  • Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1986). Differences in neuropsychological test performance associated with age, education, and sex. In I. Grant, & K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment in neuro-psychiatric disorders (pp. 100–120). Oxford University Press.
  • Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6(2), 107–128. https://doi.org/10.2307/1164588
  • Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R.. (2009). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. San Diego: Academic Press.
  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.
  • Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2004). The power of statistical tests for moderators in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(4), 426–445. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.426
  • Higby, E., Cahana-Amitay, D., Vogel-Eyny, A., Spiro IIIA., Albert, M. L., & Obler, L. K. (2019). The role of executive functions in object-and action-naming among older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 45(4), 306–330. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2019.1627492
  • Howieson, D. B., Holm, L. A., Kaye, J. A., Oken, B. S., & Howieson, J. (1993). Neurologic function in the optimally healthy oldest old: Neuropsychological evaluation. Neurology, 43(10), 1882–1886. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.43.10.1882
  • Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. (2000). The neural correlates of language production. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 845–865). MIT Press.
  • Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92(1-2), 101–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001
  • IntHout, J., Ioannidis, J. P., Borm, G. F., & Goeman, J. J. (2015). Small studies are more heterogeneous than large ones: A meta-meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 68(8), 860–869. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.03.017
  • Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G., & Petersen, R. C. (1996). Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(3), 262–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049608406689
  • Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Albert, S., Schofield, P., Dooneief, G., & Stern, Y. (1997). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment: A comparison of randomly selected, demographically matched cohorts of English-and Spanish-speaking older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 331–339. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639708403862
  • John, A., Patel, U., Rusted, J., Richards, M., & Gaysina, D. (2019). Affective problems and decline in cognitive state in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 49(3), 353–365. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001137
  • *Johns, B. T., Sheppard, C. L., Jones, M. N., & Taler, V. (2016). The role of semantic diversity in word recognition across aging and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.3389/2Ffpsyg.2016.00703.
  • Johnson, C. J., Paivio, A., & Clark, J. M. (1996). Cognitive components of picture naming. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 113–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.113
  • Jolicoeur, P., Gluck, M. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1984). Pictures and names: Making the connection. Cognitive Psychology, 16(2), 243–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(84)90009-4
  • Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston naming test. Lea and Febiger.
  • Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston naming test. Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.
  • Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1978). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febige.
  • Kavé, G., & Goral, M. (2017). Do age-related word retrieval difficulties appear (or disappear) in connected speech? Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24(5), 508–527. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1226249
  • Kavé, G., & Halamish, V. (2015). Doubly blessed: Older adults know more vocabulary and know better what they know. Psychology and Aging, 30(1), 68–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038669
  • *Kavé, G. (2005). Standardization and norms for a Hebrew naming test. Brain and Language, 92(2), 204–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.004
  • *Kavé, G., Knafo, A., & Gilboa, A. (2010). The rise and fall of word retrieval across the lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 25(3), 719–724. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018927
  • *Kavé, G., & Mashal, N. (2012). Age-related differences in word-retrieval but not in meaning generation. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19(4), 515–529. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0136).
  • *Kavé, G., Samuel-Enoch, K., & Adiv, S. (2009). The association between age and the frequency of nouns selected for production. Psychology and Aging, 24(1), 17–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014579
  • *Kavé, G., & Yafé, R. (2014). Performance of younger and older adults on tests of word knowledge and word retrieval: Independence or interdependence of skills? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(1), 36–45. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0136)
  • Kent, P. S., & Luszcz, M. A. (2002). A review of the Boston Naming Test and multiple-occasion normative data for older adults on 15-item versions. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(4), 555–574. https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.16.4.555.13916
  • *Kim, H., & Na, D. L. (1999). BRIEF REPORT normative data on the Korean version of the Boston naming test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 127–133. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.21.1.127.942
  • *LaBarge, E., Edwards, D., & Knesevich, J. W. (1986). Performance of normal elderly on the Boston Naming Test. Brain and Language, 27(2), 380–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90026-X
  • LaGrone, S., & Spieler, D. H. (2006). Lexical competition and phonological encoding in young and older speakers. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 804–809. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.804
  • *Lansing, A. E., Ivnik, R. J., Cullum, C. M., & Randolph, C. (1999). An empirically derived short form of the Boston naming test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(6), 481–487. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/14.6.481
  • Le Dorze, G., & Durocher, J. (1992). The effects of age, educational level, and stimulus length on naming in normal subjects. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 16(1), 21–29.
  • Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142
  • *Leite, K. S. B., Miotto, E. C., Nitrini, R., & Yassuda, M. S. (2017). Boston Naming Test (BNT) original, Brazilian adapted version and short forms: Normative data for illiterate and low-educated older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 29(5), 825–833. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610216001952
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525 ( 99001776
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J. (1991). The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review, 98(1), 122–142. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.1.122
  • Li, P., & Dong, Y. (2020). Language experiences and cognitive control: A dynamic perspective. In K. D. Federmeier, & H. Huang (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (72, pp. 27–52). https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2020.02.002
  • Lichtenberg, P. A., Ross, T., & Christensen, B. (1994). Preliminary normative data on the Boston Naming Test for an older urban population. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8(1), 109–111. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049408401548
  • Lövdén, M., Fratiglioni, L., Glymour, M. M., Lindenberger, U., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2020). Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 21(1), 6–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100620920576
  • Lovelace, E. A., & Twohig, P. T. (1990). Healthy older adults’ perceptions of their memory functioning and use of mnemonics. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28(2), 115–118. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03333979
  • Luchini, C., Stubbs, B., Solmi, M., & Veronese, N. (2017). Assessing the quality of studies in meta-analyses: Advantages and limitations of the Newcastle Ottawa Scale. World Journal of Meta-Analysis, 5(4), 80–84. https://doi.org/10.13105/wjma.v5.i4.80
  • Luchini, C., Veronese, N., Nottegar, A., Shin, J. I., Gentile, G., Granziol, U., Soysal, P., Alexinschi, O., Smith, L., & Solmi, M. (2021). Assessing the quality of studies in meta-research: Review/guidelines on the most important quality assessment tools. Pharmaceutical Statistics, 20(1), 185–195. https://doi.org/10.1002/pst.2068
  • Mack, W. J., Freed, D. M., Williams, B. W., & Henderson, V. W. (1992). Boston Naming Test: Shortened versions for use in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47(3), 154–158. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/47.3.p154
  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). Asymmetries in the relationship between speech perception and production. In H. Heuer & A. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 315–318). Routledge.
  • MacKay, D. G., & Burke, D. M. (1990). Cognition and aging: A theory of new learning and the use of old connections. In T. M. Hess (Ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 213–263). Elsevier Science Publisher B. V. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)60159-4
  • Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Bell, K., Merchant, C. A., Small, S., & Stern, Y. (1999). Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(3), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1017/s135561779953302x
  • Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Bell, K., Merchant, C. A., Small, S. A., & Stern, Y. (1998). Cognitive test performance among nondemented elderly African Americans and whites. Neurology, 50(5), 1238–1245. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.50.5.1238
  • *Marien, P., Mampaey, E., Vervaet, A., Saerens, J., & De Deyn, P. P. (1998). Normative data for the Boston Naming Test in native Dutch-speaking Belgian elderly. Brain and Language, 65(3), 447–467. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1998.2000
  • Mewborn, C. M., Lindbergh, C. A., & Miller, L. S. (2017). Cognitive interventions for cognitively healthy, mildly impaired, and mixed samples of older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized-controlled trials. Neuropsychology Review, 27(4), 403–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9350-8
  • *Miotto, E. C., Sato, J., Lucia, M., Camargo, C. H., & Scaff, M. (2010). Development of an adapted version of the Boston Naming Test for Portuguese speakers. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 32(3), 279–282. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-44462010005000006
  • Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press.
  • *Mitrushina, M., & Satz, P. (1995). Repeated testing of normal elderly with the Boston Naming Test. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 7(2), 123–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03324301
  • Moberg, M., Ferraro, F. R., & Petros, T. V. (2000). Lexical properties of the Boston Naming Test stimuli: Age differences in word naming and lexical decision latency. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(3), 147–153. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN0703_5
  • *Murphy, M. (2012). Normative CERAD-NP performance among community-dwelling older adults in Ireland. Clinical Gerontologist, 35(1), 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317115.2011.626514
  • Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695–699. http://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.2005.53.issue-4
  • Neils, J., Baris, J. M., Carter, C., Dell'aira, A. L., Nordloh, S. J., Weiler, E., & Weisiger, B. (1995). Effects of age, education, and living environment on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 38(5), 1143–1149. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3805.1143
  • Newman, R. S., & German, D. J. (2005). Life span effects of lexical factors on oral naming. Language and Speech, 48(2), 123–156. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309050480020101
  • Nicholas, L. E., Brookshire, R. H., Maclennan, D. L., Schumacher, J. G., & Porrazzo, S. A. (1989). Revised administration and scoring procedures for the Boston Naming Test and norms for non-brain-damaged adults. Aphasiology, 3(6), 569–580. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038908249023
  • *Nicholas, M., Obler, L., Albert, M., & Goodglass, H. (1985). Lexical retrieval in healthy aging. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 21(4), 595–606. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(58)80007-6
  • *Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Au, R., & Albert, M. L. (1996). On the nature of naming errors in aging and dementia: A study of semantic relatedness. Brain and Language, 54(2), 184–195. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0070
  • Nozari, N., & Novick, J. (2017). Monitoring and control in language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(5), 403–410. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F0963721417702419
  • Nyberg, L., Magnussen, F., Lundquist, A., Baaré, W., Bartrés-Faz, D., Bertram, L., Boraxbekk, C.-J., Brandmaier, A. M., Drevon, C. A., & Ebmeier, K. (2021). Educational attainment does not influence brain aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(18), 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2101644118
  • Opdebeeck, C., Martyr, A., & Clare, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: A meta-analysis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23(1), 40–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2015.1041450
  • Owens, C. M., & Ferron, J. M. (2012). Synthesizing single-case studies: A Monte Carlo examination of a three-level meta-analytic model. Behavior Research Methods, 44(3), 795–805. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0180-y
  • Page, M. J., Moher, D., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J. M, Hróbjartsson, A., Lalu, M. M, Li, T., Loder, E. W, Mayo-Wilson, E., McDonald, S., … McKenzie, J. E. (2021). PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. bmj, 372(71), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n160.
  • Patricacou, A., Psallida, E., Pring, T., & Dipper, L. (2007). The Boston Naming Test in Greek: Normative data and the effects of age and education on naming. Aphasiology, 21(12), 1157–1170. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030600670643
  • Peelle, J. E. (2019). Language and aging. In G. I. Zubicaray & N. O. Schiller (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 216–295). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190672027.013.12
  • Peters, J. L., Sutton, A. J., Jones, D. R., Abrams, K. R., & Rushton, L. (2007). Performance of the trim and fill method in the presence of publication bias and between-study heterogeneity. Statistics in Medicine, 26(25), 4544–4562. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2889
  • Piai, V., Roelofs, A., Acheson, D. J., & Takashima, A. (2013). Attention for speaking: Domain-general control from the anterior cingulate cortex in spoken word production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00832.
  • Piguet, O., Millar, J. L., Bennett, H. P., Lye, T. C., Creasey, H., & Broe, G. A. (2001). Boston Naming Test:: Normative data for older Australians. Brain Impairment, 2(2), 131–139. https://doi.org/10.1375/brim.2.2.131
  • Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage, 62(2), 816–847. https://doi.org/10.1016/2Fj.neuroimage.2012.04.062
  • Randolph, C., Lansing, A. E., Ivnik, R. J., Cullum, C. M., & Hermann, B. P. (1999). Determinants of confrontation naming performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(6), 489–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00023-7
  • Raymer, A. (2011). Confrontation naming. In J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, & B. Caplan (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 672–673). Springer, New York.
  • R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.
  • Reifegerste, J. (2021). The effects of aging on bilingual language: What changes, what doesn't, and why. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000413
  • Ritchie, K., Allard, M., Huppert, F. A., Nargeot, C., Pinek, B., & Ledesert, B. (1993). Computerized cognitive examination of the elderly (ECO): The development of a neuropsychological examination for clinic and population use. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 8(11), 899–914. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.930081104
  • Roberts, P. M., & Doucet, N. (2011). Performance of French-speaking Quebec adults on the Boston Naming Test. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 35(3), 254–267.
  • Rogalski, Y., Peelle, J. E., & Reilly, J. (2011). Effects of perceptual and contextual enrichment on visual confrontation naming in adult aging. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(5), 1349–1360. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0178)
  • Ross, T. P., Lichtenberg, P. A., & Christensen, B. K. (1995). Normative data on the Boston Naming Test for elderly adults in a demographically diverse medical sample. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9(4), 321–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049508400496
  • Rothman, K. J., Greenland, S., & Lash, T. L. (2008). Modern epidemiology. Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403
  • Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30(4), 507–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/2Fj.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023
  • Samu, D., Campbell, K. L., Tsvetanov, K. A., Shafto, M. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2017). Preserved cognitive functions with age are determined by domain-dependent shifts in network responsivity. Nature Communications, 8(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14743
  • Savoie, J., Root, K., Villers, J., Goldsmith, K. M., & Short, M. (2019). Boston naming test performance in French-speaking Acadians. Aphasiology, 33(5), 561–578. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2018.1490387
  • Saxton, J., Ratcliff, G., Munro, C. A., Coffey, E. C., Becker, J. T., Fried, L., & Kuller, L. (2000). Normative data on the Boston Naming Test and two equivalent 30-item short forms. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14(4), 526–534. https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.14.4.526.7204
  • *Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Vesneski, M., & Jones, D. W. R. (2000). Aging and word-finding: A comparison of spontaneous and constrained naming tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(6), 479–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6177(99)00039-6
  • Seide, S. E., Röver, C., & Friede, T. (2019). Likelihood-based random-effects meta-analysis with few studies: Empirical and simulation studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 19(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0618-3
  • Shafto, M. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2014). Language in the aging brain: The network dynamics of cognitive decline and preservation. Science, 346(6209), 583–587. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1254404
  • *Sheppard, C., Kousaie, S., Monetta, L., & Taler, V. (2016). Performance on the Boston Naming Test in bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(3), 350–363. https://doi.org/10.1017/s135561771500123x
  • *Soylu, A. E., & Cangöz, B. (2018). Adaptation and norm determination study of the Boston Naming Test for healthy Turkish elderly. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 55(4), 341–348. https://doi.org/10.5152/2Fnpa.2017.19331
  • Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(2), 461–479. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.461
  • Stang, A. (2010). Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. European Journal of Epidemiology, 25(9), 603–605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z
  • Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., & Chetelat, G., Ewers, M., Franzmeier, N., Kempermann, G., Kremen, W. S., Okonkwo, O., Scarmeas, N., Soldan, A., Udeh-Momoh, C., Valenzuela, M., Vemuri, P., Vuoksimaa, E. & Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup(2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 16(9), 1305–1311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219
  • Sterne, J. A., & Egger, M. (2005). Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borestein (Eds.), Publication bias in meta-analysis prevention, assessment and adjustments (pp. 99–110). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/0470870168.ch6
  • Sterne, J. A., & Harbord, R. M. (2004). Funnel plots in meta-analysis. The Stata Journal, 4(2), 127–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F1536867X0400400204
  • Sterne, J. A., Sutton, A. J., Ioannidis, J. P., Terrin, N., Jones, D. R., Lau, J., Carpenter, J., Rucker, G., Harbord, R. M., Schmid, C. H., Tetzlaff, J., Deeks, J. J., Peters, J., Macaskill, P., Schwarzer, G., Duval, S., Altman, D. G., Moher, D., & Higgins, J. P. (2011). Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. Bmj, 343(jul22 1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d4002.
  • Stuss, D. T., & Craik, F. I. M. (2020). Alternation in executive functions with aging. In K. M. Kenneth & S. E. Nadeau (Eds.), Cognitive changes and the aging brain (pp. 168–187). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108554350.012
  • Sullivan, M. D., Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Why is lexical retrieval slower for bilinguals? Evidence from Picture Naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 479–488. https://doi.org/10.1017/2FS1366728917000694
  • Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., & Ullman, J. B. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Pearson.
  • Theisen, M., Lerche, V., von Krause, M., & Voss, A. (2021). Age differences in diffusion model parameters: A meta-analysis. Psychological Research, 85(5), 2012–2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01371-8
  • Thuillard-Colombo , F., & Assal, G.. (1992). Adaptation française du test de dénomination de Boston versions abrégées. Revue Européenne de Psychology Appliquée, 42(1), 67–71.
  • *Tombaugh, T. N., & Hubiey, A. M. (1997). The 60-item Boston Naming Test: Norms for cognitively intact adults aged 25 to 88 years. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(6), 922–932. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639708403773
  • Torgerson, C. J. (2006). Publication bias: The Achilles’ heel of systematic reviews? British Journal of Educational Studies, 54(1), 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8527.2006.00332.x
  • Tsang, H. L., & Lee, T. M. (2003). The effect of ageing on confrontational naming ability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(1), 81–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00184-6
  • Van den Noortgate, W., López-López, J. A., Marín-Martínez, F., & & Sánchez-Meca, J. (2013). Three-level meta-analysis of dependent effect sizes. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 576–594. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0261-6
  • *Van Gorp, W. G., Satz, P., Kiersch, M. E., & Henry, R. (1986). Normative data on the Boston maming test for a group of normal older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(6), 702–705. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638608405189
  • *Verhaegen, C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Changes in naming and semantic abilities with aging from 50 to 90 years. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(2), 119–126. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617712001178
  • *Vicente, S. G., Benito-Sánchez, I., Barbosa, F., Gaspar, N., Dores, A. R., Rivera, D., & Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2022). Normative data for verbal fluency and object naming tests in a sample of European Portuguese adult population. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 29(5), 1268–1279. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2020.1868472.
  • Viechtbauer, W. (2020). Package ‘metafor’. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. Package ‘metafor’. http://www.metafor-project.org
  • *Villardita, C., Cultrera, S., Cupone, V., & Mejìa, R. (1985). Neuropsychological test performances and normal aging. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 4(4), 311–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4943(85)90038-x
  • Villasís-Keever, MÁ, Rendón-Macías, M. E., & Medina-Campos, R. H. (2018). Systematic reviews and meta-analysis in aging research. In C. García-Peña, L. Gutiérrez-Robledo, & M. Pérez-Zepeda (Eds.), Aging research-methodological issues (pp. 143–155). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-95387-8_11
  • Von Elm, E., Altman, D. G., Egger, M., Pocock, S. J., Gøtzsche, P. C., & Vandenbroucke, J. (2007). The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting. Annals of Internal Medicine, 147(8), 573–577. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-147-8-200710160-00010
  • *Welch, L. W., Doineau, D., Johnson, S., & King, D. (1996). Educational and gender normative data for the Boston Naming Test in a group of older adults. Brain and Language, 53(2), 260–266. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0047
  • Wibbelink, C. J., Hoeve, M., Stams, G. J. J., & Oort, F. J. (2017). A meta-analysis of the association between mental disorders and juvenile recidivism. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 33, 78–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2017.01.005
  • *Wierenga, C. E., Benjamin, M., Gopinath, K., Perlstein, W. M., Leonard, C. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Conway, T., Cato, M. A., Briggs, R., & Crosson, B. (2008). Age-related changes in word retrieval: Role of bilateral frontal and subcortical networks. Neurobiology of Aging, 29(3), 436–451. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709090468
  • Wilson, R. S., Yu, L., Lamar, M., Schneider, J. A., Boyle, P. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2019). Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology, 92(10), e1041–e1050. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000007036
  • Wohlin, C. (2014). Guidelines for snowballing in systematic literature studies and a replication in software engineering. In M. Shepperd, T. Hall, & I. Myrtveit (Eds.), Proceedings of the 18th international conference on evaluation and assessment in software engineering (pp. 1–10). Association for Computing Machinery.
  • Worrall, L. E., Yiu, E. M., Hickson, L. M. H., & Barnett, H. M. (1995). Normative data for the Boston Naming Test for Australian elderly. Aphasiology, 9(6), 541–551. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039508248713
  • Ylikoski, R., Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Sulkava, R., Keskivaara, P., Raininko, R., & Tilvis, R. (1998). Differences in neuropsychological functioning associated with age, education, neurological status, and magnetic resonance imaging findings in neurologically healthy elderly individuals. Applied Neuropsychology, 5(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an0501_1
  • *Zec, R. F., Burkett, N. R., Markwell, S. J., & Larsen, D. L. (2007). A cross-sectional study of the effects of age, education, and gender on the Boston Naming Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21(4), 587–616. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040701220028
  • *Zec, R. F., Markwell, S. J., Burkett, N. R., & Larsen, D. L. (2005). A longitudinal study of confrontation naming in the “normal” elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(6), 716–726. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617705050897
  • Zhang, H., Sachdev, P. S., Wen, W., Kochan, N. A., Crawford, J. D., Brodaty, H., Slavin, M. J., Reppermund, S., Kang, K., & Trollor, J. N. (2013). Grey matter correlates of three language tests in non-demented older adults. PLoS One, 8(11), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.