1,095
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Decision making and mental imagery: A conceptual synthesis and new research directions

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 603-633 | Received 08 Apr 2022, Accepted 09 Mar 2023, Published online: 05 Apr 2023

References

  • Abraham, A. (2016). The imaginative mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37(11), 4197–4211. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23300
  • Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1363–1377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
  • Aleman, A., Formisano, E., Koppenhagen, H., Hagoort, P., de Haan, E. H. F., & Kahn, R. S. (2005). The functional neuroanatomy of metrical stress evaluation of perceived and imagined spoken words. Cerebral Cortex, 15(2), 221–228. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh124
  • Amedi, A., Malach, R., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Negative BOLD differentiates visual imagery and perception. Neuron, 48(5), 859–872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.032
  • Amit, E., & Greene, J. D. (2012). You see, the ends don’t justify the means. Psychological Science, 23(8), 861–868. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434965
  • Anderson, I. M., Haddad, P. M., & Scott, J. (2012). Bipolar disorder. BMJ, 345, e8508. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.e8508
  • Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 533–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0
  • Bagozzi, R. (2008). Some insights on visual and verbal processing strategies. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(4), 258–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2008.09.003
  • Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and deciding (4th ed.). Cambridge University Press.
  • Barsics, C., Van der Linden, M., & D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Frequency, characteristics, and perceived functions of emotional future thinking in daily life. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 217–233. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1051560
  • Bartolomeo, P., Hajhajate, D., Liu, J., & Spagna, A. (2020). Assessing the causal role of early visual areas in visual mental imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 21(9), 517. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0348-5
  • Baum, J. R., Locke, E. A., & Kirkpatrick, S. A. (1998). A longitudinal study of the relation of vision and vision communication to venture growth in entrepreneurial firms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(1), 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.43
  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Nathan DeWall, C., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307301033
  • Beach, L. R. (1993). Image theory: An alternative to normative decision theory. ACR North American Advances, 20, 235–238.
  • Beach, L. R. (2009). Decision making. Narrative Inquiry, 19(2), 393–414. https://doi.org/10.1075/ni.19.2.10bea
  • Beach, L. R. (2016). A new theory of mind: The theory of narrative thought. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
  • Beach, L. R. (2021). Scenarios as narratives. Futures & Foresight Science, 3(1), ffo2.58. https://doi.org/10.1002/ffo2.58
  • Beach, L. R., & Lipshitz, R. (1993). Why classical decision theory is an inappropriate standard for evaluating and aiding most human decision making. In Decision making in action: Models and methods (pp. 21–35). Ablex Publishing.
  • Beach, L. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1987). Image theory: Principles, goals, and plans in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 66(3), 201–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(87)90034-5
  • Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior, 52(2), 336–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010
  • Belardinelli, M. O., Matteo, R. D., Gratta, C. D., Nicola, A. D., Ferretti, A., Tartaro, A., Bonomo, L., & Romani, G. L. (2004). Intermodal sensory image generation: An fMRI analysis. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(5), 729–752. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000493
  • Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice – toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 482–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011
  • Berntsen, D. (2018). The dynamics of episodic memory functions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e4. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001261
  • Berntsen, D. (2019). Spontaneous future cognitions: An integrative review. Psychological Research, 83(4), 651–665. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1127-z
  • Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1093–1104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.001
  • Birnbaum, M. H., & Chavez, A. (1997). Tests of theories of decision making: Violations of branch independence and distribution independence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71(2), 161–194. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2721
  • Bisson, J. I., Cosgrove, S., Lewis, C., & Roberts, N. P. (2015). Post-traumatic stress disorder. BMJ, 351, h6161. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.h6161
  • Blackwell, S. E. (2019). Mental imagery: From basic research to clinical practice. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 29(3), 235–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/int0000108
  • Blackwell, S. E. (2020). Emotional mental imagery. In A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (pp. 241–257). Cambridge University Press.
  • Blackwell, S. E., Dooley, D., Würtz, F., Woud, M. L., & Margraf, J. (2020). Inducing positive involuntary mental imagery in everyday life: An experimental investigation. Memory (Hove, England), 28(9), 1157–1172. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2020.1822413
  • Blais, A.-R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 33–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500000334
  • Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003
  • Brandstätter, E., Gigerenzer, G., & Hertwig, R. (2006). The priority heuristic: Making choices without trade-offs. Psychological Review, 113(2), 409–432. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.409
  • Bulley, A. (2018). The history and future of human prospection. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 2(1), 75. https://doi.org/10.26613/esic.2.1.75
  • Bulley, A., Miloyan, B., Pepper, G. V., Gullo, M. J., Henry, J. D., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Cuing both positive and negative episodic foresight reduces delay discounting but does not affect risk-taking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(8), 1998–2017. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818819777
  • Bulley, A., Redshaw, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2020). The future-directed functions of the imagination: From prediction to metaforesight. In A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (pp. 425–444). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108580298.026
  • Busemeyer, J., & Townsend, J. (1993). Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. Psychological Review, 100(3), 432–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.432
  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The rational imagination: How people create alternatives to reality. MIT Press.
  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 135–157. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249
  • Campos, A., & Pérez-Fabello, M. J. (2009). Psychometric quality of a revised version vividness of visual imagery questionnaire. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108(3), 798–802. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.108.3.798-802
  • Caruso, E. M., & Gino, F. (2011). Blind ethics: Closing one’s eyes polarizes moral judgments and discourages dishonest behavior. Cognition, 118(2), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.008
  • Chong, K., & Tuckett, D. (2015). Constructing conviction through action and narrative: How money managers manage uncertainty and the consequence for financial market functioning. Socio-Economic Review, 13(2), 309–330. https://doi.org/10.1093/ser/mwu020
  • Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive psychology interventions: Predictors and consequences of long-term positive behavior change. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(5), 355–366. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.508883
  • Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015952
  • Cokely, E. T., Galesic, M., Schulz, E., Ghazal, S., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2012). Measuring risk literacy: The Berlin numeracy test. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(1), 25–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500001819
  • Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 376–389. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0564-9
  • Crisp, R. J., Husnu, S., Meleady, R., Stathi, S., & Turner, R. N. (2010). From imagery to intention: A dual route model of imagined contact effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 188–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2010.543312
  • Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R., & Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variability can be measured objectively. Vision Research, 47(4), 474–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.11.013
  • Damasio, A. R. (2005). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. Penguin.
  • D’Argembeau, A., Raffard, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 247–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.247
  • D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van Der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96–103. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1647
  • D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.001
  • Davidson, R. J. (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biological Psychology, 67(1–2), 219–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.008
  • De Groot, K., & Thurik, R. (2018). Disentangling risk and uncertainty: When risk-taking measures are not about risk. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02194
  • Dudai, Y., & Carruthers, M. (2005). The janus face of mnemosyne. Nature, 434(7033), 567–567. https://doi.org/10.1038/434567a
  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2019). Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in reasoning and judgement. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780367823832
  • Farah, M. J. (1988). Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology. Psychological Review, 95(3), 307–317. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.307
  • Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2011). Involuntary and voluntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory (Hove, England), 19(6), 625–640. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.595722
  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39
  • Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.40.2.351
  • Fulford, J., Milton, F., Salas, D., Smith, A., Simler, A., Winlove, C., & Zeman, A. (2018). The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness – An fMRI study and literature review. Cortex, 105, 26–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.014
  • Gaesser, B., Keeler, K., & Young, L. (2018). Moral imagination: Facilitating prosocial decision-making through scene imagery and theory of mind. Cognition, 171, 180–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.004
  • Gaesser, B., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic simulation and episodic memory can increase intentions to help others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(12), 4415–4420. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1402461111
  • Ganis, G. (2020). Imagination and moral cognition. In A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (1st ed, pp. 390–405). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108580298.024
  • Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2004). Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(2), 226–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.012
  • Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Calculated risks: How to know when numbers deceive you. Simon & Schuster.
  • Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317(5843), 1351–1354. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161
  • Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press.
  • Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2008). Modeling option and strategy choices with connectionist networks: Towards an integrative model of automatic and deliberate decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(3), 215–228. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500002424
  • Glöckner, A., Betsch, T., & Schindler, N. (2010). Coherence shifts in probabilistic inference tasks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(5), 439–462. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.668
  • Glöckner, A., & Herbold, A.-K. (2011). An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(1), 71–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.684
  • Goetzmann, W. N., Kim, D., Kumar, A., & Wang, Q. (2015). Weather-induced mood, institutional investors, and stock returns. Review of Financial Studies, 28(1), 73–111. https://doi.org/10.1093/rfs/hhu063
  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872
  • Greening, S. G., Lee, T.-H., Burleigh, L., Grégoire, L., Robinson, T., Jiang, X., Mather, M., & Kaplan, J. (2022). Mental imagery can generate and regulate acquired differential fear conditioned reactivity. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 997. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05019-y
  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1310–1316. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1310
  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Gable, P. A. (2017). On the role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in approach and withdrawal motivation: An updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12879
  • Hertwig, R., & Erev, I. (2009). The description-experience gap in risky choice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 517–523. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004
  • Hertwig, R., & Wulff, D. U. (2022). A description–experience framework of the psychology of risk. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(3), 631–651. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211026896
  • Hirshleifer, D., & Shumway, T. (2003). Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. The Journal of Finance, 58(3), 1009–1032. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-6261.00556
  • Holmes, E. A., Blackwell, S. E., Burnett Heyes, S., Renner, F., & Raes, F. (2016). Mental imagery in depression: Phenomenology, potential mechanisms, and treatment implications. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 249–280. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-092925
  • Holmes, E. A., Geddes, J. R., Colom, F., & Goodwin, G. M. (2008). Mental imagery as an emotional amplifier: Application to bipolar disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(12), 1251–1258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.09.005
  • Holmes, E. A., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2006). Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces. Neuroscience Letters, 394(1), 48–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.002
  • Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing interpretation bias modification as a "cognitive vaccine" for depressed mood: Imagining positive events makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally.. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 76–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012590
  • Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: A special relationship? Emotion, 5(4), 489–497. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.489
  • Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(3), 349–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001
  • Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion, 8(3), 395–409. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.395
  • Hoppe, J. M., Holmes, E. A., & Agren, T. (2021). Exploring the neural basis of fear produced by mental imagery: Imaginal exposure in individuals fearful of spiders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1817), 20190690. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0690
  • Hsee, C. K., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2004). Music, pandas, and muggers: On the affective psychology of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 23–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.23
  • Huber, O., Beutter, C., Montoya, J., & Huber, O. W. (2001). Risk-defusing behaviour: Towards an understanding of risky decision making. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 409–426. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440125915
  • Isen, A. M., & Labroo, A. A. (2003). Some ways in which positive affect facilitates decision making and judgment. In S. L. Schneider, & J. Shanteau (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research (pp. 365–393). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511609978.013
  • Ishai, A., & Sagi, D. (1995). Common mechanisms of visual imagery and perception. Science, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7792605
  • Ivins, A., Di Simplicio, M., Close, H., Goodwin, G. M., & Holmes, E. (2014). Mental imagery in bipolar affective disorder versus unipolar depression: Investigating cognitions at times of ‘positive’ mood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 166, 234–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.007
  • Ji, J. L., Heyes, S. B., MacLeod, C., & Holmes, E. A. (2016). Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: Fear and beyond—a tribute to peter lang. Behavior Therapy, 47(5), 702–719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.004
  • Johnson, S., & Tuckett, D. (2017). Narrative decision-making in investment choices: How investors use news about company performance. SSRN Electronic Journal, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3037463
  • Kahneman, D., Ritov, I., & Schkade, D. (1999). Economic preferences or attitude expressions?: An analysis of dollar responses to public issues. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19(1/3), 203–235. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007835629236
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty (1st ed, pp. 201–208). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511809477.015
  • Kahneman, D., Wakker, P. P., & Sarin, R. (1997). Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 375–406. https://doi.org/10.1162/003355397555235
  • Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A., & Levi, M. D. (2003). Winter blues: A SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review, 93(1), 324–343. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282803321455322
  • Kappes, H. B., & Morewedge, C. K. (2016). Mental simulation as substitute for experience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(7), 405–420. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12257
  • Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112(2), 446–467. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.446
  • Keeney, R. L., & Raiffa, H. (1993). Decisions with multiple objectives: Preferences and value tradeoffs. Cambridge University Press.
  • Keltner, D., & Lerner, J. S. (2010). Emotion. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 253–352). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1039/pc9797600253
  • Keogh, R., & Pearson, J. (2018). The blind mind: No sensory visual imagery in aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.012
  • Kim, S.-E., Kim, J.-W., Kim, J.-J., Jeong, B. S., Choi, E. A., Jeong, Y.-G., Kim, J. H., Ku, J., & Ki, S. W. (2007). The neural mechanism of imagining facial affective expression. Brain Research, 1145, 128–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.048
  • Klein, G. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50(3), 456–460. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872008X288385
  • Klein, I., Dubois, J., Mangin, J.-F., Kherif, F., Flandin, G., Poline, J.-B., Denis, M., Kosslyn, S. M., & Le Bihan, D. (2004). Retinotopic organization of visual mental images as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(1), 26–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.07.006
  • Knudstrup, M., Segrest, S. L., & Hurley, A. E. (2003). The use of mental imagery in the simulated employment interview situation. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18(6), 573–591. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940310494395
  • Knutson, B., & Huettel, S. A. (2015). The risk matrix. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 141–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.10.012
  • Koffka, K. (1934). Principles of gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. MIT Press.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635–642. https://doi.org/10.1038/35090055
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N. M. (1997). Neural systems shared by visual imagery and visual perception: A positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage, 6(4), 320–334. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1997.0295
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Klm, I. J., & Alpert, N. M. (1995). Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature, 378(6556), 496–498. https://doi.org/10.1038/378496a0
  • Kossowska, M., Letki, N., Zaleskiewicz, T., & Wichary, S. (2022). Human behaviour in pandemics: Social and psychological determinants in a global health crisis. Routledge.
  • Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2000). Imagery neurons in the human brain. Nature, 408(6810), 357–361. https://doi.org/10.1038/35042575
  • Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., & Kozak, M. J. (1983). Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92(3), 276–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.92.3.276
  • Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., Strong, D. R., & Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The balloon analogue risk task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75
  • Lempert, K. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Neuroeconomics of emotion and decision making. In P. Glimcher, & E. Fehr (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain: Second edition (pp. 219–236). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-416008-8.00012-7
  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146
  • Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 799–823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043
  • Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). Oxford University Press.
  • Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 573–597. https://doi.org/10.2307/2118482
  • Loewenstein, G., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267
  • Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. The Economic Journal, 92(368), 805–824. https://doi.org/10.2307/2232669
  • Lopes, L., & Oden, G. (1999). The role of aspiration level in risky choice: A comparison of cumulative prospect theory and SP/A theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 43(2), 286–313. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmps.1999.1259
  • Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. (1989). Games and decisions: Introduction and critical survey. Dover Publications.
  • MacKisack, M., Aldworth, S., Macpherson, F., Onians, J., Winlove, C., & Zeman, A. (2016). On picturing a candle: The prehistory of imagery science. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00515
  • MacLeod, A. (1994). Worry and explanation-based pessimism. In Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 115–134). John Wiley & Sons.
  • MacLeod, A. (1999). Prospective cognitions. In T. Dalgleish, & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 267–280). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/0470013494.ch14
  • MacLeod, A. (2017). Prospection, well-being, and mental health. Oxford University Press.
  • Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., Muldoon, M. F., & Ferrell, R. E. (2003). A neurobiology of intertemporal choice. In Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice (pp. 139–172). Russell Sage Foundation.
  • Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of Psychology, 64(1), 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x
  • Marks, D. F. (1999). Consciousness, mental imagery and action. British Journal of Psychology, 90(4), 567–585. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712699161639
  • Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., & Holmes, E. A. (2013). Feels like the real thing: Imagery is both more realistic and emotional than verbal thought. Cognition & Emotion, 27(2), 217–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.698252
  • May, J., Kavanagh, D. J., & Andrade, J. (2015). The elaborated intrusion theory of desire: A 10-year retrospective and implications for addiction treatments. Addictive Behaviors, 44, 29–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.09.016
  • McClelland, J. L., Mirman, D., Bolger, D. J., & Khaitan, P. (2014). Interactive activation and mutual constraint satisfaction in perception and cognition. Cognitive Science, 38(6), 1139–1189. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12146
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
  • Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8(6), 423–429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455.x
  • Mellet, E., Tzourio, N., Denis, M., & Mazoyer, B. (1995). A positron emission tomography study of visual and mental spatial exploration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(4), 433–445. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1995.7.4.433
  • Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., & Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic foresight and anxiety: Proximate and ultimate perspectives. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 4–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12080
  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244(4907), 933–938. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056
  • Mishra, S. (2014). Decision-making under risk. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 280–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314530517
  • Mohr, P. N. C., Biele, G., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Neural processing of risk. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(19), 6613–6619. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-10.2010
  • Montgomery, H. (1983). Advances in psychology. Advances in Psychology, 14, 343–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62243-8
  • Montgomery, H. (1989). From cognition to action: The search for dominance in decision making. In H. Montgomery, & O. Svenson (Eds.), Process and structure in human decision making (pp. 23–49). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
  • Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1273–1280. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0314
  • Murphy, S. M., & Martin, K. A. (2002). The use of imagery in sport. In Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed, pp. 405–439). Human Kinetics.
  • Murphy, S. M., Nordin, S., & Cumming, J. (2008). Imagery in sport, exercise, and dance. In Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed, pp. 297–324, 463–467). Human Kinetics.
  • Nanay, B. (2016). The role of imagination in decision-making. Mind & Language, 31(1), 127–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12097
  • Neck, C. P., & Manz, C. C. (1992). Thought self-leadership: The influence of self-talk and mental imagery on performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(7), 681–699. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030130705
  • Neck, C. P., & Manz, C. C. (1996). Thought self-leadership: The impact of mental strategies training on employee cognition, behavior, and affect. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17(5), 445–467. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199609)17:5<445::AID-JOB770>3.0.CO;2-N
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483–522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
  • Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., Kawashima, R., Fukuda, H., Itoh, M., & Yamadori, A. (2003). Thinking of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1369–1380. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00179-4
  • Ottaviani, R., & Beck, A. T. (1987). Cognitive aspects of panic disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1(1), 15–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-6185(87)90019-3
  • Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195066661.001.0001
  • Parrott, W. G. (2017). Role of emotions in risk perception. In G. Emilien, R. Weitkunat, & F. Lüdicke (Eds.), Consumer perception of product risks and benefits (pp. 221–232). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-50530-5_12
  • Pearson, J. (2019). The human imagination: The cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 624–634. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0202-9
  • Pearson, J., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). The heterogeneity of mental representation: Ending the imagery debate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(33), 10089–10092. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504933112
  • Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 590–602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.003
  • Peters, E., & Slovic, P. (1996). The role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions in the perception and acceptance of nuclear Power1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(16), 1427–1453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1996.tb00079.x
  • Pfister, H., & Böhm, G. (2008). The multiplicity of emotions: A framework of emotional functions in decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(1), 5–17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500000127
  • Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16(2), 331–348. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1087
  • Phelps, E. A., Lempert, K. M., & Sokol-Hessner, P. (2014). Emotion and decision making: Multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1), 263–287. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014119
  • Poppa, T., & Bechara, A. (2018). The somatic marker hypothesis: Revisiting the role of the ‘body-loop’ in decision-making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.10.007
  • Pratt, D., Cooper, M. J., & Hackmann, A. (2004). Imagery and its characteristics in people who are anxious about spiders. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32(2), 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465804001158
  • Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Harvard University Press.
  • Ramsey, R., Cumming, J., & Edwards, M. G. (2009). Mental imagery inflates performance expectations but not actual performance of a novel and challenging motor task. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 28(4), 331–347. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.28.4.d
  • Raune, D., MacLeod, A., & Holmes, E. A. (2005). The simulation heuristic and visual imagery in pessimism for future negative events in anxiety. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 12(4), 313–325. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.455
  • Read, S. J., Vanman, E. J., & Miller, L. C. (1997). Connectionism, parallel constraint satisfaction processes, and gestalt principles: (Re)Introducing cognitive dynamics to social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1(1), 26–53. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_3
  • Renner, F., Murphy, F. C., Ji, J. L., Manly, T., & Holmes, E. A. (2019). Mental imagery as a “motivational amplifier” to promote activities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 114, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.02.002
  • Reyna, V. F., & Huettel, S. A. (2014). Reward, representation, and impulsivity: A theoretical framework for the neuroscience of risky decision making. In The neuroscience of risky decision making (pp. 11–42). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14322-002
  • Reyna, V. F., Nelson, W. L., Han, P. K., & Dieckmann, N. F. (2009). How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 943–973. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017327
  • Reyna, V. F., Wilhelms, E. A., McCormick, M. J., & Weldon, R. B. (2015). Development of risky decision making: Fuzzy-trace theory and neurobiological perspectives. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 122–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12117
  • Rick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (2008). Intangibility in intertemporal choice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 3813–3824. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0150
  • Riquelme, H. (2002). Can people creative in imagery interpret ambiguous figures faster than people less creative in imagery? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(2), 105–116. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2002.tb01059.x
  • Rottenstreich, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2001). Money, kisses, and electric shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12(3), 185–190. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00334
  • Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1997). Unpacking, repacking, and anchoring: Advances in support theory. Psychological Review, 104(2), 406–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.406
  • Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574–586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574
  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001
  • Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., & Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic future thinking: Mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.002
  • Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2009). Comparing mental imagery across the sensory modalities. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 28(4), 371–388. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.28.4.g
  • Schoemaker, P. J. H. (1982). The expected utility model: Its variants, purposes, evidence and limitations. Journal of Economic Literature, 20(2), 529–563.
  • Schulze, C., & Hertwig, R. (2021). A description–experience gap in statistical intuitions: Of smart babies, risk-savvy chimps, intuitive statisticians, and stupid grown-ups. Cognition, 210, 104580. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104580
  • Schwarz, N. (2000). Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 433–440. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402745
  • Schwarz, N. (2001). Feelings as information: Implications for affective influences on information processing. In L. L. Martin, & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s handbook (pp. 159–176). Erlbaum.
  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513–523. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513
  • Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Homo prospectus. Oxford University Press.
  • Seligman, M. E. P., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., & Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 119–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612474317
  • Shah, A. K., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Heuristics made easy: An effort-reduction framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 207–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.207
  • Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature, 450(7166), 102–105. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06280
  • Sheffrin, S. M. (1996). Rational expectations (2nd ed). Cambridge University Press.
  • Simon, D., Stenstrom, D. M., & Read, S. J. (2015). The coherence effect: Blending cold and hot cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(3), 369–394. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000029
  • Sinclair, A. H., Manalili, G. M., Brunec, I. K., Adcock, R. A., & Barense, M. D. (2021). Prediction errors disrupt hippocampal representations and update episodic memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(51), e2117625118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2117625118
  • Sjöberg, L. (2007). Emotions and risk perception. Risk Management, 9(4), 223–237. https://doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.rm.8250038
  • Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177(3), 1333–1352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejor.2005.04.006
  • Slovic, P., Flynn, J. H., & Layman, M. (1991). Perceived risk, trust, and the politics of nuclear waste. Science, 254(5038), 1603–1607. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.254.5038.1603
  • Slovic, P., & Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 322–325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00461.x
  • Smieja, J. M., Zaleskiewicz, T., Sobkow, A., & Traczyk, J. (2023). Imagining risk taking: The valence of mental imagery Is related to the declared willingness to take risky actions. PsyArXiv, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/kyv4p
  • Smith, R., & Lane, R. D. (2016). Unconscious emotion: A cognitive neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 216–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.013
  • Sobkow, A., Traczyk, J., & Zaleskiewicz, T. (2016). The affective bases of risk perception: Negative feelings and stress mediate the relationship between mental imagery and risk perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(932), https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00932
  • Stallen, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2015). The neuroscience of social conformity: Implications for fundamental and applied research. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 337. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00337
  • Starmer, C. (2000). Developments in non-expected utility theory: The hunt for a descriptive theory of choice under risk. Journal of Economic Literature, 38(2), 332–382. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.38.2.332
  • Stein, M. B., & Gorman, J. (2001). Unmasking social anxiety disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 26(3), 185–189.
  • Stevenson, L. (2003). Twelve conceptions of imagination. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 43(3), 238–259. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjaesthetics/43.3.238
  • Suddendorf, T. (2013). The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. Basic Books, a member of the Perseus Books Group.
  • Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2003). Mental time travel in animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 391–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00187-6
  • Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36(2), 110–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.010
  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123(2), 133–167.
  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(03), 299–313. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001975
  • Svenson, O. (1992). Differentiation and consolidation theory of human decision making: A frame of reference for the study of pre- and post-decision processes. Acta Psychologica, 80(1–3), 143–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(92)90044-E
  • Switras, J. E. (1978). An alternate-form instrument to assess vividness and controllability of mental imagery in seven modalities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46(2), 379–384. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.46.2.379
  • Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(2), 142–162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610362350
  • Szpunar, K. K., Spreng, R. N., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(52), 18414–18421. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417144111
  • Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53(4), 429–439. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.429
  • Tetlock, P. E., & Gardner, D. (2015). Superforecasting: The art and science of prediction (1st ed.). Crown Publishers.
  • Thagard, P., & Millgram, E. (1995). Inference to the best plan: A coherence theory of decision. In A. Ram, & D. B. Leake (Eds.), Goal-driven learning (pp. 439–454). MIT Press.
  • Tobler, P. N., & Weber, E. U. (2013). Valuation for risky and uncertain choices. In P. Glimcher, & E. Fehr (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain: Second edition (pp. 149–172). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-416008-8.00009-7
  • Traczyk, J., Sobkow, A., & Zaleskiewicz, T. (2015). Affect-laden imagery and risk taking: The mediating role of stress and risk perception. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0122226. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122226
  • Tuckett, D., & Nikolic, M. (2017). The role of conviction and narrative in decision-making under radical uncertainty. Theory & Psychology, 27(4), 501–523. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354317713158
  • Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114
  • Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79(4), 281–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032955
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297–323. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574
  • Tversky, A., & Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101(4), 547–567. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.547
  • Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., Crockett, M. J., Crum, A. J., Douglas, K. M., Druckman, J. N., Drury, J., Dube, O., Ellemers, N., Finkel, E. J., Fowler, J. H., Gelfand, M., Han, S., Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460–471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
  • Volz, K. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2012). Cognitive processes in decisions under risk are not the same as in decisions under uncertainty. Frontiers in Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00105
  • von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press.
  • Vrana, S. R., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1986). Fear imagery and text processing. Psychophysiology, 23(3), 247–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00626.x
  • Weber, E. U. (2017). Understanding public risk perception and responses to changes in perceived risk. In E. J. Balleisen, J. B. Wiener, K. D. Krawiec, & L. S. Bennear (Eds.), Policy shock: Recalibrating risk and regulation after oil spills, nuclear accidents and financial crises (pp. 58–81). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316492635.004
  • Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131–134. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00355.x
  • Winkielman, P., & Berridge, K. C. (2004). Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 120–123. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00288.x
  • Winlove, C. I. P., Milton, F., Ranson, J., Fulford, J., MacKisack, M., Macpherson, F., & Zeman, A. (2018). The neural correlates of visual imagery: A co-ordinate-based meta-analysis. Cortex, 105, 4–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.014
  • Wyer, R. S., Hung, I. W., & Jiang, Y. (2008). Visual and verbal processing strategies in comprehension and judgment. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(4), 244–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2008.09.002
  • Zaleskiewicz, T., & Traczyk, J. (2020). Emotions and financial decision making. In T. Zaleskiewicz, & J. Traczyk (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on financial decision making (pp. 107–133). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-45500-2_6
  • Zaleskiewicz, T., Traczyk, J., Sobkow, A., Fulawka, K., & Megías-Robles, A. (2022). Visualizing risky behaviors induces a stronger neural response in brain areas responsible for mental imagery and emotions than visualizing neutral behaviors. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-1189673/v1
  • Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., Manstead, A. S. R., & vanr de Pligt, J. (2000). On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: The psychology of regret and disappointment. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 521–541. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402781
  • Zeman, A. (2020). Aphantasia. In A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (1st ed, pp. 692–710). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108580298.042

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.