70
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Language-specific cognitive flexibility is related to code-switching habits and interactional context; domain-general cognitive flexibility is not

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Received 21 Aug 2023, Accepted 03 Jun 2024, Published online: 17 Jun 2024

References

  • Adler, R. M., Valdés Kroff, J. R., & Novick, J. M. (2020). Does integrating a code-switch during comprehension engage cognitive control? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(4), 741–759. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000755
  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). The MIT Press.
  • Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Massonnié, J., Flitton, A., Kirkham, N., & Evershed, J. K. (2020). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 388–407. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x
  • Barbu, C. A., Gillet, S., & Poncelet, M. (2020). Investigating the effects of language-switching frequency on attentional and executive functioning in proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1078. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01078
  • Barbu, C., Orban, S., Gillet, S., & Poncelet, M. (2018). The impact of language switching frequency on attentional and executive functioning in proficient bilingual adults. Psychologica Belgica, 58(1), 115–127. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.392
  • Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., & Dussias, P. E. (2020a). Codeswitching: A bilingual toolkit for opportunistic speech planning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1699. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01699
  • Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2020b). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(6), 1022–1047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770
  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
  • Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L. (2017). Bilingual language switching in the laboratory versus in the wild: The spatiotemporal dynamics of adaptive language control. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(37), 9022–9036. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0553-17.2017
  • Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
  • Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39(4), 713–726. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00466-5
  • Chan, C. G., Yow, W. Q., & Oei, A. (2020). Active bilingualism in aging: Balanced bilingualism usage and less frequent language switching relate to better conflict monitoring and goal maintenance ability. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(9), e231–e241. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa058
  • Claussenius-Kalman, H. L., Vaughn, K. A., Archila-Suerte, P., & Hernandez, A. E. (2021). Highly proficient, balanced bilingualism is related to thinner cortex in two cognitive control regions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1485(1), 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14491
  • Colzato, L. S., Van Leeuwen, P. J., Van Den Wildenberg, W., & Hommel, B. (2010). DOOM'd to switch: Superior cognitive flexibility in players of first person shooter games. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00008
  • Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2000). The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1283–1296. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1283
  • Cox, J. G., LaBoda, A., & Mendes, N. (2020). “I'm gonna Spanglish it on you”: Self-reported vs. oral production of Spanish–English codeswitching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000129
  • Dass, R., Smirnova-Godoy, I., McColl, O., Grundy, J. G., Luk, G., & Anderson, J. A. (2024). A Content Overlap Analysis of bilingualism questionnaires: Considering diversity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000767
  • De Baene, W., Duyck, W., Brass, M., & Carreiras, M. (2015). Brain circuit for cognitive control is shared by task and language switching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(9), 1752–1765. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00817
  • Declerck, M., Koch, I., Duñabeitia, J. A., Grainger, J., & Stephan, D. N. (2019). What absent switch costs and mixing costs during bilingual language comprehension can tell us about language control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(6), 771–789. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000627
  • DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A., & Krott, A. (2020). Understanding bilingual brain function and structure changes? U bet! A unified bilingual experience trajectory model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100930
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Wereley, S. (2022). Investigation of code-switching cost in conversation and self-paced reading tasks. International Journal of Bilingualism, 26(3), 308–333. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069211056438
  • Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Méot, A., Augustinova, M., & Pallier, C. (2010). The French Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 488–496. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.488
  • Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6(1), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-5
  • Freeman, M. R., Robinson Anthony, J. J., Marian, V., & Blumenfeld, H. K. (2022). Individual and sociolinguistic differences in language background predict Stroop performance. Frontiers in Communication, 7, 105. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2022.865965
  • Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodríguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernandez, M., Costa, A., & Ávila, C. (2010). Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. Neuroimage, 53(4), 1272–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078
  • Goldfarb, L., & Henik, A. (2007). Evidence for task conflict in the Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1170–1176. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1170
  • Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S. (2009). Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 640–665. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014981
  • Gosselin, L., & Sabourin, L. (2021). Lexical-semantic processing costs are not inherent to intra-sentential code-switching: The role of switching habits. Neuropsychologia, 159, 107922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107922
  • Gosselin, L., & Sabourin, L. (2023). Language athletes: Dual-language code-switchers exhibit inhibitory control advantages. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1150159. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1150159
  • Grainger, J., & Beauvillain, C. (1987). Language blocking and lexical access in bilinguals. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 39(2), 295–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748708401788
  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
  • Grundy, J. G. (2020). The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: An updated quantitative analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 4(2), 177–199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00062-5
  • Guerra-Carrillo, B., Katovich, K., & Bunge, S. A. (2017). Does higher education hone cognitive functioning and learning efficacy? Findings from a large and diverse sample. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182276. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182276
  • Gullifer, J. W., Kroll, J. F., & Dussias, P. E. (2013). When language switching has no apparent cost: Lexical access in sentence context. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 278. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00278
  • Han, X., Wei, L., & Filippi, R. (2022). Modulating effects of interactional contexts on bilinguals’ cognitive control: Evidence for the Adaptive Control Hypothesis. International Journal of Bilingualism, 27, 548–568. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069221102011
  • Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016
  • Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2020). The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 609–633. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000672
  • Hofweber, J., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2019). Predicting executive functions in bilinguals using ecologically valid measures of code-switching behaviour. In I. A. Sekerina, L. Spradlin, & V. Valian (Eds.), Bilingualism, Executive Functions, and Beyond. Questions and Insights (pp. 181–205). Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.57
  • Hofweber, J. E., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2020a). Experimentally induced language modes and regular code-switching habits boost bilinguals’ executive performance: Evidence from a within-subject paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 542326. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.542326
  • Hofweber, J., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2020b). How different code-switching types modulate bilinguals’ executive functions: A dual control mode perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(4), 909–925. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000804
  • Howard, G. S. (1994). Why do people say nasty things about self-reports? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15(5), 399–404. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030150505
  • Hui, N. Y., Fong, M. C. M., & Wang, W. S. (2022). Bilingual prefabs: No switching cost was found in cantonese–english habitual code-switching in Hong Kong. Languages, 7(3), 198. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030198
  • Jevtović, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & de Bruin, A. (2020). How do bilinguals switch between languages in different interactional contexts? A comparison between voluntary and mandatory language switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 401–413. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000191
  • Jiang, S., Ma, L., & Chen, B. (2023). Dynamic engagement of cognitive control in intra-sentential code-switching during comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 62–77. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000323
  • Jiao, L., Grundy, J. G., Liu, C., & Chen, B. (2020). Language context modulates executive control in bilinguals: Evidence from language production. Neuropsychologia, 142, 107441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107441
  • Jiao, L., Liu, C., Liang, L., Plummer, P., Perfetti, C. A., & Chen, B. (2019). The contributions of language control to executive functions: From the perspective of bilingual comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(8), 1984–1997. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818821601
  • Jylkkä, J., Laine, M., & Lehtonen, M. (2021). Does language switching behavior rely on general executive functions? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(3), 583–595. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000619
  • Jylkkä, J., Lehtonen, M., Lindholm, F., Kuusakoski, A., & Laine, M. (2018). The relationship between general executive functions and bilingual switching and monitoring in language production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 505–522. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000104
  • Jylkkä, J., Soveri, A., Laine, M., & Lehtonen, M. (2020). Assessing bilingual language switching behavior with Ecological Momentary Assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 309–322. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918001190
  • Jylkkä, J., Soveri, A., Wahlström, J., Lehtonen, M., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2017). Relationship between language switching experience and executive functions in bilinguals: An Internet-based study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 404–419. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1282489
  • Kałamała, P., Chuderski, A., Szewczyk, J., Senderecka, M., & Wodniecka, Z. (2022). Bilingualism caught in a net: A new approach to understanding the complexity of bilingual experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
  • Kałamała, P., Szewczyk, J., Chuderski, A., Senderecka, M., & Wodniecka, Z. (2020). Patterns of bilingual language use and response inhibition: A test of the adaptive control hypothesis. Cognition, 204, 104373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104373
  • Kalanthroff, E., Avnit, A., Henik, A., Davelaar, E. J., & Usher, M. (2015). Stroop proactive control and task conflict are modulated by concurrent working memory load. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 869–875. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0735-x
  • Kechu, O., & Som, B. (2023). Is task specific control among balanced bilinguals a result of nature of acquisition and language use? Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 7(1), 23–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-022-00114-y
  • Korenar, M., Treffers-Daller, J., & Pliatsikas, C. (2023a). Two languages in one mind: Insights into cognitive effects of bilingualism from usage-based approaches. Naše řeč, 106, 24–46.
  • Korenar, M., Treffers-Daller, J., & Pliatsikas, C. (2023b). Dynamic effects of bilingualism on brain structure map onto general principles of experience-based neuroplasticity. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 3428. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-30326-3
  • Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 126–147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.15.1.126
  • Lai, G., & O’Brien, B. A. (2020). Examining language switching and cognitive control through the adaptive control hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1171. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01171
  • Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142
  • Li, X., Ng, K. K., Wong, J. J. Y., Lee, J. W., Zhou, J. H., & Yow, W. Q. (2021). Bilingual language entropy influences executive functions through functional connectivity and signal variability. Brain and Language, 222, 105026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105026
  • Lira, B., O’Brien, J. M., Peña, P. A., Galla, B. M., D'Mello, S., Yeager, D. S., Kautz, T., Munkacsy, K., & Duckworth, A. L. (2022). Large studies reveal how reference bias limits policy applications of self-report measures. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 19189. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-23373-9
  • Lo, S., & Andrews, S. (2015). To transform or not to transform: Using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1171. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01171
  • López-Penadés, R., Sanchez-Azanza, V. A., Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Buil-Legaz, L., & Adrover-Roig, D. (2020). Associations of natural language switching with executive control in early bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 1008–1019. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000012
  • Marin-Marin, L., Costumero, V., Ávila, C., & Pliatsikas, C. (2022). Dynamic effects of immersive bilingualism on cortical and subcortical grey matter volumes. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 886222. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886222
  • Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90(2), 227–234. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031564
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Molinaro, N., Nara, S., & Carreiras, M. (2023). Early language dissociation in the bilingual mind: Sensor-level MEG evidence. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.03.22.533825
  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R. D., Moll, F. T., Bramati, I. E., & Andreiuolo, P. A. (2002). The cerebral correlates of set-shifting: An fMRI study of the trail making test. Arquivos de Neuro-psiquiatria, 60(4), 900–905. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2002000600002
  • Mosca, M., & Clahsen, H. (2016). Examining language switching in bilinguals: The role of preparation time. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(2), 415–424. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000693
  • Muysken, P. (2000). Bilingual speech: A typology of code-mixing. Cambridge University Press.
  • Navarro, E., DeLuca, V., & Rossi, E. (2022). It takes a village: Using network science to identify the effect of individual differences in bilingual experience for Theory of Mind. Brain Sciences, 12(4), 487. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040487
  • Nichols, E. S., Wild, C. J., Stojanoski, B., Battista, M. E., & Owen, A. M. (2020). Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: A population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychological Science, 31(5), 548–567. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620903113
  • Olson, D. J. (2022). The Bilingual Code-Switching Profile (BCSP): Assessing the reliability and validity of the BCSP questionnaire. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 14(3), 400–433. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.21039.ols
  • Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From bilingualism to bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(4), 867–879. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000020
  • Paap, K. R. (2023). The bilingual advantage in executive functioning hypothesis : How the debate provides insight into psychology’s replication crisis (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003308027.
  • Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mikulinsky, R., Masuda, S., & Mason, L. (2019). On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.12.001
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
  • Paap, K. R., Mason, L., & Anders-Jefferson, R. (2021). Predictions about the cognitive consequences of language switching on executive functioning inspired by the adaptive control hypothesis fail more often than not. Brain Sciences, 11(9), 1217. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091217
  • Paap, K. R., Myuz, H. A., Anders, R. T., Bockelman, M. F., Mikulinsky, R., & Sawi, O. M. (2017). No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(2), 89–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436
  • Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 598–609. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.598
  • Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(27), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093840
  • Pliatsikas, C. (2020). Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 459–471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000130
  • Plonsky, L., & Ghanbar, H. (2018). Multiple regression in L2 research: A methodological synthesis and guide to interpreting R2 values. The Modern Language Journal, 102(4), 713–731. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12509
  • Poplack, S. (1980). Sometimes I’ll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en español: Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics, 18(7-8), 581–618. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1980.18.7-8.581
  • Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682–691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000580
  • Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2013). The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 622–645. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
  • Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526
  • Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Kramer, U., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Festman, J., & Münte, T. F. (2012). Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 388. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00388
  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
  • Rossi, E., Dussias, P. E., Diaz, M., van Hell, J. G., & Newman, S. (2021). Neural signatures of inhibitory control in intra-sentential code-switching: Evidence from fMRI. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 57, 100938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100938
  • Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1477–1491. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1477
  • Sabourin, L., Brien, C., & Burkholder, M. (2014). The effect of age of L2 acquisition on the organization of the bilingual lexicon: Evidence from masked priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 542–555. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000643
  • Sabourin, L., Leclerc, J.-C., Lapierre, M., Burkholder, M., & Brien, C. (2016). The language background questionnaires in L2 research: Teasing apart the variables. In L. Hracs (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2016 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistics Association.
  • Sanchez-Azanza, V. A., López-Penadés, R., Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., & Adrover-Roig, D. (2020). Latent variable evidence on the interplay between language switching frequency and executive control in Spanish-Catalan bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(5-6), 912–930. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006920902525
  • Savin, N. E., & White, K. J. (1977). The Durbin-Watson test for serial correlation with extreme sample sizes or many regressors. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 45, 1989–1996. doi:10.2307/1914122
  • Shen, Q. (2023). Investigating the modulation of active preparation and passive dissipation on inhibitory control processes in the language switching paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1065268. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1065268
  • Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
  • Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183
  • Stasenko, A., Matt, G. E., & Gollan, T. H. (2017). A relative bilingual advantage in switching with preparation: Nuanced explorations of the proposed association between bilingualism and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(11), 1527–1550. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000340
  • Stuss, D. T., Bisschop, S. M., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., Katz, D., & Izukawa, D. (2001). The trail making test: A study in focal lesion patients. Psychological Assessment, 13(2), 230–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.13.2.230
  • Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401–415. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000682
  • Treffers-Daller, J., Ongun, Z., Hofweber, J., & Korenar, M. (2020). Explaining individual differences in executive functions performance in multilinguals: The impact of code-switching and alternating between multicultural identity styles. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 561088. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.561088
  • van den Berg, F., Brouwer, J., Tienkamp, T. B., Verhagen, J., & Keijzer, M. (2022). Language entropy relates to behavioral and pupil indices of executive control in young adult bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1760. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.864763
  • Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2012). Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27(4), 959–974. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028281
  • Xu, Q., Markowska, M., Chodorow, M., & Li, P. (2021). Modeling bilingual lexical processing through code-switching speech: A network science approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 662409. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.662409
  • Yeh, L. H., & Lim, J. Y. (2021). Multilinguals in a monolingual community: The influence of non-linguistic switching cost and awareness of language environment on intentional and unintentional language switch. Revista Linguagem em Foco, 13(4), 78–95. https://doi.org/10.46230/2674-8266-13-7354
  • Yim, O., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Degree of conversational code-switching enhances verbal task switching in Cantonese–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000478
  • Yow, W. Q., & Li, X. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 164. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00164
  • Yow, W. Q., Oei, A. C., & Li, X. (2017). Novel evidence for the bilingual advantage: Effects of language control on executive function in balanced and unbalanced dual-language users. 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.
  • Zhu, J. D., Blanco-Elorrieta, E., Sun, Y., Szakay, A., & Sowman, P. F. (2022). Natural vs forced language switching: Free selection and consistent language use eliminate significant performance costs and cognitive demands in the brain. NeuroImage, 247, 118797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118797

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.