602
Views
26
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Cultural influences on the teleological stance: evidence from China

, , , , &
Pages 17-26 | Received 24 Jul 2015, Accepted 06 Nov 2015, Published online: 22 Feb 2016

References

  • Adler, J.A. (2005). Chinese religion: An overview. In L. Jones (Ed.), Encyclopedia of religion (2nd ed., pp. 1580–1613). Detroit, MI: Macmillan Reference.
  • Anderson, D.L., Fisher, K.M., & Norman, G.J. (2002). Development and evaluation of the conceptual inventory of natural selection. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 952–978. doi: 10.1002/tea.10053
  • Aristotle. (2008). Physics. (R. Waterfield, Trans.) New York: Oxford University Press. (Original work published c. 350 BC).
  • Association of Religion Data Archives. (2007). Spiritual life study of Chinese residents. Retrieved from http://www.thearda.com/Archive/Files/Codebooks/SPRTCHNA_CB.asp
  • Atran, S. (1995). Causal constraints on categories and categorical constraints on biological reasoning across cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A.J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 205–233). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Banerjee, K., & Bloom, P. (2013). Would Tarzan believe in God? Conditions for the emergence of religious belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 7–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.005
  • Banerjee, K., & Bloom, P. (2014). Why did this happen to me? Religious believers’ and non-believers’ teleological reasoning about life events. Cognition, 133(1), 277–303. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.017
  • Barrett, J.L. (2012). Born believers: The science of children's religious belief. New York: Free Press.
  • Bering, J. (2010). Atheism is only skin deep: Geertz and Markússon rely mistakenly on sociodemographic data as meaningful indicators of underlying cognition. Religion, 40(3), 166–168. doi: 10.1016/j.religion.2009.11.001
  • Bering, J. (2011). The belief instinct: The psychology of souls, destiny, and the meaning of life. New York: Norton.
  • Bloom, P. (2007). Religion is natural. Developmental Science, 10(1), 147–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00577.x
  • Casler, K., & Kelemen, D. (2008). Developmental continuity in teleo-functional explanation: Reasoning about nature among Romanian Romani adults. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 340–362. doi: 10.1080/15248370802248556
  • Clark, K.J., & Winslett, J.T. (2011). The evolutionary psychology of Chinese religion: Pre-Qin High Gods as punishers and rewarders. Journal of the American Academy of Religion, 79(4), 928–960. doi: 10.1093/jaarel/lfr018
  • Cohen, J.D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25, 257–271. doi: 10.3758/BF03204507
  • Coley, J.D. (2000). On the importance of comparative research: The case of folkbiology. Child Development, 71(1), 82–90. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00121
  • Davis, W.E., Juhl, J., & Routledge, C. (2011). Death and design: The terror management function of teleological beliefs. Motivation and Emotion, 35(1), 98–104. doi: 10.1007/s11031-010-9193-6
  • Diesendruck, G., & Haber, L. (2009). God's categories: The effect of religiosity on children's teleological and essentialist beliefs about categories. Cognition, 110(1), 100–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.001
  • DiYanni, C., & Kelemen, D. (2005). Time to get a new mountain? The role of function in children's conceptions of natural kinds. Cognition, 97(3), 327–335. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.002
  • Evans, E.M. (2001). Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 42(3), 217–266. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0749
  • Evans, J.St.B.T., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(4), 382–389. doi: 10.1080/13546780542000005
  • Geertz, A.W., & Markússon, G.I. (2010). Religion is natural, atheism is not: On why everybody is both right and wrong. Religion, 40(3), 152–165. doi: 10.1016/j.religion.20
  • Gelman, S.A., & Kremer, K.E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children's explanations of how objects and their properties originate. Child Development, 62(2), 396–414. doi: 10.2307/1131012
  • Gelman, S.A., Mannheim, B., Escalante, C., & Sánchez Tapia, I. (in press). Teleological talk in parent-child conversations in Quechua. First Language.
  • Gervais, W.M., Willard, A.K., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious beliefs. Religion, 41(3), 389–410. doi: 10.1080/0048721X.2011.604510
  • Goldberg, R.F., & Thompson-Schill, S.L. (2009). Developmental “roots” in mature biological knowledge. Psychological Science, 20(4), 480–487. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02320.x
  • Harris, P., & Koenig, M. (2006). Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77(3), 505–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x
  • Heine, S.J., & Norenzayan, A. (2006). Toward a psychological science for a cultural species. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 251–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00015.x
  • Heywood, B.T., & Bering, J.M. (2013). “Meant to be”: How religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 4(3), 183–201. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2013.782888
  • Järnefelt, E., Canfield, C.F., & Kelemen, D. (2015). The divided mind of a disbeliever: Intuitive beliefs about nature as purposefully created among different groups of non-religious adults. Cognition, 140, 72–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.02.005
  • Ji, L.-J., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R.E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 57–65. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57
  • Keil, F.C. (1992). The origins of an autonomous biology. In M. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in language and cognition: The Minnesota symposia (pp. 103–137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Kelemen, D. (1999a). Beliefs about purpose: On the origins of teleological thought. In M. Corballis & S. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind (pp. 278–294). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Kelemen, D. (1999b). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70, 241–272. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4
  • Kelemen, D. (1999c). Why are rocks pointy? Children's preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1440–1452. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1440
  • Kelemen, D. (2003). British and American children's preferences for teleo-functional explanations of the natural world. Cognition, 88(2), 201–221. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00024-6
  • Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists?” Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295–301. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00672.x
  • Kelemen, D. (2012). Teleological minds: How natural intuitions about agency and purpose influence learning about evolution. In K.S. Rosengren, S.K. Brem, E.M. Evans, & G.M. Sinatra (Eds.), Evolution challenges: Integrating research and practice in teaching and learning about evolution (pp. 66–92). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Kelemen, D., & DiYanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children's reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(1), 3–31. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0601_2
  • Kelemen, D., & Rosset, E. (2009). The human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111(1), 138–143. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.001
  • Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2013). Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1074–1083. doi: 10.1037/a0030399
  • Lagerway, J., & Kalinowski, M. (Eds.). (2011). Early Chinese religion, part one: Shang through Han (1250 BC-220 AD). Leiden, Holland: Brill.
  • Legare, C.H., Evans, E.M., Rosengren, K.S., & Harris, P.L. (2012). The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Development, 83(3), 779–793. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01743.x
  • Libarkin, J.C., & Anderson, S.W. (2006). The Geoscience Concept Inventory. In X. Liu & W.J. Boone (Eds.), Applications of Rasch measurement in science education (pp. 45–73). Fort Dodge, IA: JAM Publishers.
  • Lombrozo, T., & Carey, S. (2006). Functional explanation and the function of explanation. Cognition, 99(2), 167–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.009
  • Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D., & Zaitchik, D. (2007). Inferring design: Evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Science, 18(11), 999–1006. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02015.x
  • Ma, L., & Xu, F. (2013). Preverbal infants infer intentional agents from the perception of regularity. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1330–1337. doi: 10.1037/a0029620
  • Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
  • Mills, R., & Frowley, J. (2015). Promiscuous teleology and the effect of locus of control. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 35, 121–132. doi: 10.1080/03033910.2015.1011192
  • Newman, G.E., Keil, F.C., Kuhlmeier, V.A., & Wynn, K. (2010). Early understandings of the link between agents and order. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(40), 17140–17145. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914056107
  • Nisbett, R.E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. New York: Free Press.
  • ojalehto, B., Waxman, S.R., & Medin, D.L. (2013). Teleological reasoning about nature: Intentional design or relational perspectives? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.006
  • Petrovich, O. (1997). Understanding of non-natural causality in children and adults: A case against artificialism. Psyche en Geloof, 8, 151–165.
  • Rottman, J., & Kelemen, D. (2012). Is there such a thing as a Christian child? Evidence of religious beliefs in early childhood. In P. McNamara & W.J. Wildman (Eds.), Science and the world's religions (Vol. 2, pp. 205–238). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
  • Sánchez Tapia, I., Gelman, S.A., Hollander, M.A., Manczak, E.M., Mannheim, B., & Escalante, C. (in press). Development of teleological explanations in Peruvian Quechua-speaking and U.S. English-speaking preschoolers and adults. Child Development.
  • Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124(2), 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005
  • Slingerland, E., & Chudek, M. (2011). The prevalence of mind-body dualism in Early China. Cognitive Science, 35(5), 997–1007. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01186.x
  • Smith, T.W. (2012). Beliefs about God across time and countries. Retrieved from http://publicdata.norc.org:41000/gss/documents//CNRT/Godissp.pdf
  • Stark, R., & Liu, E.Y. (2011). The religious awakening in China. Review of Religious Research, 52(3), 282–289.
  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
  • Tickle, P.A. (1997). God-talk in America. New York: Crossroads.
  • Willard, A.K., & Norenzayan, A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life's purpose. Cognition, 129(2), 379–391. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016
  • WIN-Gallup International. (2012). Global index of religiosity and atheism. Retrieved from http://www.wingia.com/web/files/news/14/file/14.pdf
  • Yang, F. (2004). Between secularist ideology and desecularizing reality: The birth and growth of religious research in communist China. Sociology of Religion, 65(2), 101–119. doi: 10.2307/3712401
  • Yang, F. (2011). Religion in China: Survival and revival under communist rule. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Yang, F., & Hu, A. (2012). Mapping Chinese folk religion in Mainland China and Taiwan. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 51(3), 505–521. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01660.x
  • Zuckerman, P. (2008). Society without God: What the least religious nations can tell us about contentment. New York: New York University Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.