1,141
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Special Section: Representing mental representations: Neuroscientific and computational approaches to information processing in the brain

Effect size matters: the role of language statistics and perceptual simulation in conceptual processing

, , &

References

  • Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning & Memory, 16, 248–266. doi:10.1101/lm.918309
  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x
  • Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Bergen, B. K. (2012). Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Beilock, S. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesture changes thought by grounding it in action. Psychological Science, 21, 1605–1610. doi:10.1177/0956797610385353
  • Beilock, S. L., & Holt, L. E. (2007). Embodied preference judgments: Can likeability be driven by the motor system? Psychological Science, 18(1), 51–57. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x
  • Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science, 222, 1029–1031. doi:10.1126/science.6648516
  • Bergen, B. K., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 733–764. doi:10.1080/03640210701530748
  • Borghi, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Memory & Cognition, 32, 863–873. doi:10.3758/BF03196865
  • Borreggine, K. L., & Kaschak, M. P. (2006). The action-sentence compatibility effect: It's all in the timing. Cognitive Science, 30, 1097–1112. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_91
  • Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M., & Genevro, J. L. (2000). Child language with mother and with stranger at home and in the laboratory: A methodological study. Journal of Child Language, 27, 407–420. doi:10.1017/S0305000900004165
  • Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 355–363. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020
  • Burman, D. D., Bitan, T., & Booth, J. R. (2008). Sex differences in neural processing of language among children. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1349–1362. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.021
  • Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., & Pezaris, E. (2001). Spatial-mechanical reasoning skills versus mathematics self-confidence as mediators of gender differences on mathematics subtests using cross-national gender-based items. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 32(1), 28–57. doi:10.2307/749620
  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.
  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489–509.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2013). Flexible and fast: Linguistic shortcut affects both shallow and deep conceptual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 542–550. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0368-x
  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2009). Is a bear white in the woods? Parallel representation of implied object color during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 573–577. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.3.573
  • Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Dreyer, F. (2012). A functional role for modality-specific perceptual systems in conceptual representations. PLoS One, 7, e33321. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033321.t001
  • Davies, C. (2013). Reading geography between the lines: Extracting local place knowledge from text. In T. Tenbrink, J. Stell, A. Galton, & Z. Wood (Eds.), Conference on spatial information theory (COSIT) (pp. 320–337). Scarborough, UK: Springer.
  • de Saussure, F. (1916). Cours de linguistique générale (C. Bally & A. Sechehaye, eds.), trans. W. Baskin, Course in General Linguistics, Glasgow: Fontana/Collins, 1977.
  • de Vega, M., Glenberg, A. M., & Graesser, A. C. (Eds.). (2008). Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Dijkstra, K., Yaxley, R. H., Madden, C. J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The role of age and perceptual symbols in language comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 19, 352–356. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.352
  • Gagné, C. L. (2002). Lexical and relational influences on the processing of novel compounds. Brain and Language, 81, 723–735. doi:10.1006/brln.2001.2559
  • Estes, Z. (2003). Attributive and relational processes in nominal combination. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 304–319. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00507-7
  • Estes, Z., Verges, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects’ typical location. Psychological Science, 19, 93–97. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02051.x
  • Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 11–15. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00158
  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The language of thought revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2–18. doi:10.1037/a0024338
  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual system. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.
  • Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–596.
  • Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28(1), 1–26. doi:10.1080/01638539909545067
  • Glenberg, A. M., Becker, R., Klötzer, S., Kolanko, L., Müller, S., & Rinck, M. (2009). Episodic affordances contribute to language comprehension. Language and Cognition, 1, 113–135. doi:10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.006
  • Glenberg, A. M., Robertson, D. A., Jansen, J. L., & Johnson-Glenberg, M. C. (1999). Not propositions. Cognitive Systems Research, 1(1), 19–33. doi:10.1016/S1389-0417(99)00004-2
  • Greenberg, J. (1966). Language universals, with special reference to feature hierarchies. The Hague: Mouton.
  • Hauk, O., & Tschentscher, N. (2013). The body of evidence: What can neuroscience tell us about embodied semantics? Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00050
  • Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., & Rinck, M. (2007). Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 436–441. doi:10.3758/BF03194085
  • Holt, L. E., & Beilock, S. L. (2006). Expertise and its embodiment: Examining the impact of sensorimotor skill expertise on the representation of action-related text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 694–701. doi:10.3758/BF03193983
  • Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2012). The upbeat of language: Linguistic context and embodiment predict processing of valence words. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1709–1714). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. (2013a). Language statistics and individual differences in processing primary metaphors. Cognitive Linguistics, 24, 667–687. doi:10.1515/cog-2013-0023
  • Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2013b). Language statistics explain the spatial–numerical association of response codes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 470–478. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0492-2
  • Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2013c). What's up can be explained by language statistics. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Washsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2596–2601). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2010). Temperature perceptions as a ground for social proximity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 867–873. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.015
  • Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D., & Schubert, T. W. (2009). Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20, 1169–1174. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02426.x
  • Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 93, 1449–1475. doi:10.1257/000282803322655392
  • Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94, B79–B89. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005
  • Kaschak, M. P., Zwaan, R. A., Aveyard, M., & Yaxley, R. H. (2006). Perception of auditory motion affects language processing. Cognitive Science, 30, 733–744. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_54
  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kaup, B., Lüdtke, J., & Maienborn, C. (2010). “The drawer is still closed”: Simulating past and future actions when processing sentences that describe a state. Brain and Language, 112, 159–166. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.08.009
  • Kaup, B., Yaxley, R. H., Madden, C. J., Zwaan, R. A., & Lüdtke, J. (2007). Experiential simulations of negated text information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 976–990. doi:10.1080/17470210600823512
  • Koch, S., Holland, R. W., Hengstler, M., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Body locomotion as regulatory process: stepping backward enhances cognitive control. Psychological Science, 20, 549–550. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02342.x
  • Knott, A. (2012). Sensorimotor cognition and natural language syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
  • Landauer, T. K., McNamara, D. S., Dennis, S., & Kintsch, W. (2007). Handbook of latent semantic analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Liu, Y., Wang, F., Kang, C., Gao, Y., & Lu, Y. (2014). Analyzing relatedness by toponym co-occurrences on web pages. Transactions in GIS (Geographic Information Systems), 18, 89–107.
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2007). Symbolic embodied representations: A case for symbol interdependency. In T. Landauer, D. McNamara, S. Dennis, & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Handbook of latent semantic analysis (pp. 107–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 838–844. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.4.838
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011a). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. TopiCS in Cognitive Science, 3, 273–302. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011b). Stormy seas and cloudy skies: conceptual processing is (still) linguistic and perceptual. Frontiers in Psychology, 1–4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00105/full
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Benesh, N. (2012). Representing spatial structure through maps and language: Lord of the Rings encodes the spatial structure of Middle Earth. Cognitive Science, 36, 1556–1569. doi:10.1111/cogs.12000
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Connell, L. (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35, 381–398. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01157.x
  • Louwerse, M. M. & Hutchinson, S. (2012). Neurological evidence linguistic processes precede perceptual simulation in conceptual processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 385. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00385
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114, 96–104. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.002
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2008). How fundamental is embodiment to language comprehension? Constraints of embodied cognition. In V. Sloutsky, B. Love, & K. McRae (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1313–1318). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 51–73. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2008.01003.x
  • Louwerse, M. M., Hutchinson, S., & Cai, Z. (2012). The Chinese route argument: Predicting the longitude and latitude of cities in China and the Middle East using statistical linguistic frequencies. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society Proceedings of the 34th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 695–700). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C. M. (2005). Constraining theories of embodied cognition. Psychological Science, 16(1), 6–10. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00772.x
  • Matlock, T. (2004). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1389–1400. doi:10.3758/BF03206329
  • Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243–247. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00659.x
  • Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., & Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What's ‘up’ with God? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 699–710. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.699
  • Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82–87. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502002.x
  • Meteyard, L., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2007). Motion detection and motion verbs: Language affects low-level visual perception. Psychological Science, 18, 1007–1013. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02016.x
  • Meteyard, L., Zokaei, N., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words. Current Biology, 18, R732–R733. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.016
  • Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9
  • Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M. H., & Fitneva, S. A. (2011). The arbitrariness of the sign: Learning advantages from the structure of the vocabulary. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 325–347. doi:10.1037/a0022924
  • Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. C., Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2014). How arbitrary is language? Philosophical. Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130299. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0299
  • Myung, J.-Y., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: Manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98, 223–243. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.010
  • Nuthmann, A., & van der Meer, E. (2005). Time's arrow and pupillary response. Psychophysiology, 42, 306–317. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00291.x
  • Paivio A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
  • Pecher, D., van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Zanolie, K., & Huber, D. E. (2010). Congruency between word position and meaning is caused by task-induced spatial attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00030
  • Pecher, D., van Dantzig, S., Zwaan, R. A., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009). Language comprehenders retain implied shape and orientation of objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1108–1114. doi:10.1080/17470210802633255
  • Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.). (2005). Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Pedhazur, E. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  • Rapp, D. N., & Horton, W. S. (2003). Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 104–110.
  • Recchia, G., & Louwerse, M. M. (2014). Grounding the ungrounded: Estimating locations of unknown place names from linguistic associations and grounded representations. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1270–1275). Austin, TX: The Cognitive Science Society.
  • Recchia, G. (2014, September 8). Computationally estimating geographical information from user-contributed data Presented at NARP (NGA Academic Research Program Symposium and Workshops) 2014, Washington, DC.
  • Richardson,D., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102(1), 129–138. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.004
  • Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767–780. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2705_4
  • Richter, T., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Processing of color words activates color representations. Cognition, 111, 383–389. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.011
  • Roberts, J. K., & Monaco, J. P. (2006). Effect size measures for the two-level linear multilevel model. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.
  • Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2012). Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Santana, E., & de Vega, M. (2011). Metaphors are embodied, and so are their literal counterparts. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00090
  • Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516. doi:10.3758/BF03194099
  • Santos, A., Chaigneau, S. E., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Property generation reflects word association and situated simulation. Language & Cognition, 3(1), 83–119. doi:10.1515/langcog.2011.004
  • Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1–21. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1
  • Sell, A. J., & Kaschak, M. P. (2010). Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain and Language, 117, 39–44. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.003
  • Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.). (2008). Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Šetic, M., & Domijan, D. (2007). The influence of vertical spatial orientation on property verification. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 297–312. doi:10.1080/01690960600732430
  • Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied cognition. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Simmons, W. K., Hamann, S. B., Harenski, C. N., Hu, X. P., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). fMRI evidence for word association and situated simulation in conceptual processing. Journal of PhysiologyParis, 102, 106–119.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior (pp. 53–54). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Solomon, K. O., & Barsalou, L. W. (2004). Perceptual simulation in property verification. Memory & Cognition, 32, 244–259. doi:10.3758/BF03196856
  • Stanfield, R. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12, 153–156. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00326
  • Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–665.
  • Tagalakis, G., & Keane, M. T. (2006). Familiarity and relational preference in the understanding of noun-noun compounds. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1285–1297. doi:10.3758/BF03193272
  • Taylor, L., Lev-Ari, S., & Zwaan,R. (2008). Inferences about action engage action systems. Brain and Language, 107(1), 62–67. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.08.004
  • Tillman, R. N., Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2013). Geographical estimates are explained by perceptual simulation and language statistics. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 3557–3562). Austin, TX: The Cognitive Science Society.
  • Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Perceptual processing affects conceptual processing. Cognitive Science, 32, 579–590. doi:10.1080/03640210802035365
  • van Dantzig, S., Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2009). Unconstraining theories of embodied cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 345–351. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.001
  • Vermeulen, N., Mermillod, M., Godefroid, J., & Corneille, O. (2009). Unintended embodiment of concepts into percepts: Sensory activation boosts attention for same-modality concepts in the attentional blink paradigm. Cognition, 112, 467–472. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.003
  • Wilson, A. D. & Golonka, S. (2013). Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Frontiers in Psychology 4, 58. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058
  • Yang, F., Mo, L., Louwerse, M. M. (2012). Effects of local and global context on processing sentences with subject and object relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 227–237.
  • Wu, L.-l., & Barsalou, L.W. (2009). Perceptual simulation in conceptual combination: Evidence from property generation. Acta Psychologica, 132, 173–189. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.02.002
  • Yaxley, R. H. & Zwaan, R. A. (2006). Simulating visibility during language comprehension. Cognition, 105, 229–236.
  • Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol.44, pp. 35–62). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 954–958. doi:10.3758/BF03196557
  • Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., Yaxley, R. H., & Aveyard, M. E. (2004). Moving words: Dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28, 611–619. doi:10.1016/j.cogsci.2004.03.004
  • Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13, 168–171. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00430
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1–11. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.