644
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Ageing effects on word production processes: an ERP topographic analysis

&
Pages 1259-1272 | Received 14 Apr 2014, Accepted 29 May 2015, Published online: 14 Aug 2015

References

  • Abdel Rahman, R., van Turennout, M., & Levelt, J. W. M. (2003). Phonological encoding is not contingent on semantic feature retrieval: An electrophysiological study on object naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 850–860.
  • Abrams, L., White, K. K., & Eitel, S. L. (2003). Isolating phonological components that increase tip-of-the-tongue resolution. Memory and Cognition, 31, 1153–1162. doi: 10.3758/BF03195798
  • Alario, F. X., Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (2002). Hedging one's bets too much? A reply to Levelt (2002). Language & Cognitive Processes, 17(6), 673–682. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000461
  • Alario, F. X., & Ferrand, L. (1999). A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 531–552. doi: 10.3758/BF03200732
  • Alario, F. X., Ferrand, L., Laganaro, M., New, B., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Segui, J. (2004). Predictors of picture naming speed. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 140–155. doi: 10.3758/BF03195559
  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data. A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Balota, D. A., & Duchek, J. M. (1988). Age-related differences in lexical access, spreading activation, and simple pronunciation. Psychology and Aging, 3, 84–93. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.3.1.84
  • Barresi, B. A., Nicholas, M., Connor, L. T., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (2000). Semantic degradation and lexical access in age-related naming failures. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section B), 7(3), 169–178. doi: 10.1076/1382-5585(200009)7:3;1-Q;FT169
  • Bates, D. M., & Sarkar, D. (2007). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.9975–12.
  • Beier, M. E., & Ackerman, P. L. (2001). Current-events knowledge in adults: An investigation of age, intelligence, and nonability determinants. Psychology & Aging, 16, 615–628. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.615
  • Belke, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2007). Single and multiple object naming in healthy ageing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 1178–1211. doi:10.1080/01690960701461541
  • Bonin, P., Peerman, R., Malardier, N., Méot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158–167. doi: 10.3758/BF03195507
  • Bortfeld, H., Leon, S. D., Bloom, J. E., Schober, M. F., & Brennan, S. E. (2001). Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role, and gender. Language and Speech, 44(2), 123–147. doi: 10.1177/00238309010440020101
  • Brunet, D., Murray, M. M., & Michel, C. M. (2011). Spatiotemporal analysis of multichannel EEG: CARTOOL. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, Article ID 813870. doi:10.1155/2011/813870
  • Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., & James, L. E. (2000). Theoretical approaches to language and aging. In T. Perfect & E. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 204–237). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults?. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 542–579. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90026-G
  • Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2004). Aging and language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 21–24. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301006.x
  • Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2008). Language and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373–443). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046743
  • Ceponiene, R., Westerfield, M., Torki, M., & Townsend, J. (2008). Modality-specificity of sensory aging in vision and audition: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1215, 53–68. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.010
  • Changeux, J.-P., & Michel, C. M. (2004). Mechanisms of neural integration at the brain scale level: The neuronal workspace and microstate models. In: S. Grillner & A. M. Grabyel (Eds.), Microcircuits: The interface between neurons and global brain function (pp. 347–370). Dahlem Workshop Report. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Connor, L. T., Spiro, A., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (2004). Change in object naming ability during adulthood. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(5), P203–P209. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.5.P203
  • Cutting, J. C., & Ferreira, V. S. (1999). Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 318–344.
  • Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 313–349. doi: 10.1080/01690969008407066
  • Evrard, M. (2002). Aging and lexical access to common and proper names in picture naming. Brain and Language, 81, 174–179. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2515
  • Falkenstein, M., Yordanova, J., & Kolev, V. (2006). Effects of aging on slowing of motor-response generation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 59, 22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.08.004
  • Feyereisen, P. (1997). A meta-analytic procedure shows an age related decline in picture naming: Comments on Goulet, Ska, and Kahn (1994). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1328–1333. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1328
  • Feyereisen, P., Demaeght, N., & Samson, D. (1998). Why do picture naming latencies increase with age: General slowing, greater sensitivity to interference, or task-specific deficits? Experimental Aging Research, 24, 21–51. doi: 10.1080/036107398244346
  • Galdo-Alvarez, S., Lindín, M., & Díaz, F. (2009). The effect of age on event-related potentials (ERP) associated with face naming and with the tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) state. Biological Psychology, 81, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.01.002
  • Gazzaley, A., Clapp, W., Kelley, J., McEvoy, K., Knight, R. T., & D'Esposito, M. (2008). Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13122–13126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806074105
  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61–105. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-O
  • Goral, M., Spiro, A. I., Albert, M. L., Obler, L. K., & Connor, L. T. (2007). Change in lexical retrieval skills in adulthood. Mental Lexicon, 2, 215–238. doi: 10.1075/ml.2.2.05gor
  • Goulet, P., Ska, B., & Kahn, H. J. (1994). Is there a decline in picture naming with advancing age? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(3), 629–644. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3703.629
  • Hopfinger, J. B., & Mangun, G. R. (1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychological Science, 9, 441–447. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00083
  • Indefrey, P. (2011). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255
  • Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92, 101–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
  • James, L. E., & Burke, D. M. (2000). Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1378–1391.
  • Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824–843.
  • Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., & Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 463–492. doi: 10.1080/02643290701674851
  • Knobel, M., Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2008). The many places of frequency: Evidence for a novel locus of the lexical frequency effect in word production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(2), 256–286. doi: 10.1080/02643290701502425
  • Kuznetsova, A. (2014). Package ‘lmerTest’: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package).
  • Laganaro, M., & Perret, C. (2011). Comparing electrophysiological correlates of word production in immediate and delayed naming through the analysis of word age of acquisition effects. Brain Topography, 24, 19–29. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0162-x
  • Laganaro, M., Valente, A., & Perret, C. (2012). Time course of word production in fast and slow speakers: A high density ERP topographic study. Neuroimage, 59, 3881–3888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.082
  • Laver, G. D., & Burke, D. M. (1993). Why do semantic priming effects increase in old age? A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8, 34–43. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.1.34
  • Lehmann, D., & Skrandies, W. (1984). Spatial analysis of evoked potentials in man – A review. Progress in Neurobiology, 23, 227–250. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(84)90003-0
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.
  • Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 432–440. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01545-X
  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • MacKay, D. G., & Burke, D. M. (1990). Cognition and aging: A theory of new learning and the use of old connections. In T. Hess (Ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 213–263). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Michel, C. M., Thut, G., Morand, S., Khateb, A., Pegna, A. J., & Grave de Peralta, R. (2001). Electric source imaging of human brain functions. Brain Research Reviews, 36, 108–118. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00086-8
  • Miozzo, M., Pulvermüller, F., & Hauk, O. (2014). Early parallel activation of semantics and phonology in picture naming: Evidence from a multiple-linear-regression MEG study. Cerebral Cortex, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu137
  • Morrison, C. M., Hirsh, K. W., & Duggan, G. B. (2003). Age of acquisition, ageing and verb production: Normative and experimental data. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(A), 2–26.
  • Mortensen, L., Meyer, A. S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2006). Age-related effects on speech production: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(1), 238–290. doi: 10.1080/01690960444000278
  • Murray, M. M., Brunet, D., & Michel, C. (2008). Topographic ERP analyses, a step-by step a tutorial review. Brain Topography, 20, 249–264. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5
  • Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.475
  • Navarrete, E., Basagni, B., Alario, F. X., & Costa, A. (2006). Does word frequency affect lexical selection is speech production? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1681–1690. doi: 10.1080/17470210600750558
  • Neumann, Y., Obler, L. K., Gomes, H., & Shafer, V. (2009). Phonological vs. sensory contributions to age effects in naming: An electrophysiological study. Aphasiology, 23 (7), 1028–1039. doi: 10.1080/02687030802661630
  • New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 516–524. doi: 10.3758/BF03195598
  • Nicholas, M., Connor, L. T., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1998). Aging, language, and language disorders. In M. T. Sarno (Ed.), Acquired aphasia (pp. 413–449). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann, D. (1995). Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates, model estimation and validation. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 42, 658–665. doi: 10.1109/10.391164
  • Protopapas, A. (2007). Check vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 859–862. doi: 10.3758/BF03192979
  • Rabbitt, P., Maylor, E., McInnes, L., Bent, N., & Moore, B. (1995). What goods can self-assessment questionnaires deliver for cognitive gerontology? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, S127–S152. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350090709
  • Rastle, K. G., & Burke, D. M. (1996). Priming the tip of the tongue: Effects of prior processing on word retrieval in young and older adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 586–605. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0031
  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403
  • Santiago, J., MacKay, D. G., Palma, A., & Rho, C. (2000). Sequential activation processes in producing words and syllables: Evidence from picture naming. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 1–44. doi: 10.1080/016909600386101
  • Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Shafto, M. A., Stamatakis, E. A., Tam, P. P., & Tyler, L. K. (2010). Word retrieval failures in old age: The relationship between structure and function. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1530–1540. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21321
  • Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8, 1–120.
  • Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Damian, M. F., Pérez, M. A., Bowers, J. S., & Marín, J. (2009). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a large set of pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1581–1597. doi: 10.1080/17470210802511139
  • Stothart, G., Tales, A., Hedge, C., & Kazanina, N. (2014). Double peaked P1 visual evoked potentials in healthy ageing. Clinical Neurophysiology, 125, 1471–1478. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.11.029
  • Strijkers, K., Costa, A., & Thierry, G. (2010). Tracking lexical access in speech production: Electrophysiological correlates of word frequency and cognate effects. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 912–928. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp153
  • Taylor, J. K., & Burke, D. M. (2002). Asymmetric aging effects on semantic and phonological processes: Naming in the picture-word interference task. Psychology and Aging, 17, 662–676. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.662
  • Theios, J., & Amrhein, P. C. (1989). Theoretical analysis of the cognitive processing of lexical and pictorial stimuli: Reading, naming, and visual and conceptual comparisons. Psychological Review, 96, 5–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.5
  • Thornton, R., & Light, L. L. (2006). Language comprehension and production in normal aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 261–287). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
  • Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 19(2), 203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8
  • Verhaegen, C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Changes in naming and semantic abilities with aging from 50 to 90 years. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 119–126. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001178
  • White, K. K., & Abrams, L. (2002). Does priming specific syllables during tip-of-the tongue states facilitate word retrieval in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 226–235. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.226
  • Zacks, R. T., & Hasher, L. (1997). Cognitive gerontology and attentional inhibition: A reply to Burke and McDowd. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52B, P274–P283. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52B.6.P274

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.