1,488
Views
36
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Introduction

Cross-linguistic psycholinguistics and its critical role in theory development: early beginnings and recent advances

, & ORCID Icon
Pages 1009-1032 | Received 21 Jul 2015, Accepted 31 Jul 2015, Published online: 16 Oct 2015

References

  • Abney, S. P. (1989). A computational model of human parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 129–144. doi:10.1007/BF01069051
  • Aksu-Koç, A. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1985). Acquisition of Turkish. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1: The data (pp. 839–878). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.
  • Aksu-Koç, A. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1986). A psychological account of the development and use of evidentials in Turkish. In W. Chafe & J. Nichols (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology (pp. 159–167). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Allen, S. E. M. (1996). Aspects of argument structure acquisition in Inuktitut. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/lald.13
  • Ammon, M. S., & Slobin, D. I. (1979). A cross-linguistic study of the processing of causative sentences. Cognition, 7, 3–17. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(79)90007-6
  • Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R. (2000). Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 76(1), 28–55. doi: 10.1353/lan.2000.0045
  • Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Asudeh, A., & Alrenga, P. (2004). Avoiding attachment ambiguities: The role of constituent ordering. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 55–70. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.006
  • Aylett, M., & Turk, A. (2004). The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 47(1), 31–56. doi:10.1177/00238309040470010201
  • Bates, E., Devescovi, A., & Wulfeck, B. (2001). Psycholinguistics: A cross-language perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 369–396. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.369
  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 173–218). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 157–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney & E. Bates (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3–73). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A., & Smith, S. (1982). Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study. Cognition, 11, 245–299. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(82)90017-8
  • Bavin, E., & Stoll, S. (Eds.). (2013). The acquisition of ergativity [Trends in Language Acquisition series (TILAR)]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/tilar.9
  • Bavin, E. L. (1992). The acquisition of Walpiri. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 309–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Berman, R. A. (2014). Cross-linguistic comparisons in child language research. Journal of Child Language, 41, 26–37. doi:10.1017/S0305000914000208
  • Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–352). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Bickel, B., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., Choudhary, K. K., Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2015). The neuophysiology of language processing shapes the evolution of grammar: Evidence from case marking. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0132819. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0132819
  • Bleek, Wilhelm H. I. (1851). De nominum generibus linguarum Africae Australia. Bonnae: Formis Caroli Georgii.
  • Bock, K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review, 89(1), 1–47. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.89.1.1
  • Bock, K. (1990). Structure in language: Creating form in talk. American Psychologist, 45, 1221–1236. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.11.1221
  • Bock, J. K., & Ferreira, V. (2014). Syntactically speaking. In M. Goldrick, V. Ferreira, & M. Miozzo (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Production. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Bock, J. K., Irwin, D. E., & Davidson, D. J. J. (2004). Putting first things first. In F. Ferreira & M. Henderson (Eds.), The integration of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 249–278). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Bohnemeyer, J. B., Butler, L. K., & Jaeger, T. F. (in press). Head-marking and agreement: Evidence from Yucatec Maya. For Van Valin Festschrift.
  • Bornkessel, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2006). The extended argument dependency model: A neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages. Psychological Review, 113(4), 787. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.787
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Choudhary, K. K., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., & Bickel, B. (2008). Bridging the gap between processing preferences and typological distributions: Initial evidence from the online comprehension of control constructions in Hindi. Scales (= Ling. Arbeits Berichte 86), pp. 397–436.
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Kretzschmar, F., Tune, S., Wang, L., Genç, S., Philipp, M., & Schlesewsky, M. (2011). Think globally: Cross-linguistic variation in electrophysiological activity during sentence comprehension. Brain and Language, 117(3), 133–152. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.09.010
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). Minimality as vacuous distinctness: Evidence from cross-linguistic sentence comprehension. Lingua, 119(10), 1541–1559. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2008.03.005
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). The role of prominence information in the real-time comprehension of transitive constructions: A cross-linguistic approach. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 19–58. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00099.x
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Neurotypology: Modeling crosslinguistic similarities and differences in the neurocognition of language comprehension. In M. Sanz, I. Laka, & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Language down the garden path: The cognitive and biological basis of linguistic structure (pp. 241–252). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199677139.003.0012
  • Boston, M. F., Hale, J. T., Vasishth, S., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Parallel processing and sentence comprehension difficulty. Language & Cognitive Processes, 26(3), 301–349. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.492228
  • Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: A cross linguistic study, with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bowerman, M. (1985). What shapes children's grammars? In D. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (pp. 1257–1319). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Bowerman, M. (2011). Linguistic typology and first language acquisition. In J. J. Song (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic typology (pp. 591–617). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Brown, P. (1998). Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category. Linguistics, 36(4), 713–753. doi:10.1515/ling.1998.36.4.713
  • Brysbaert, M., & Mitchell, D. C. (1996). Modifier attachment in sentence parsing: Evidence from Dutch. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 664–695. doi:10.1080/713755636
  • Butler, L. K., Bohnemeyer, J. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2014). Plural marking in Yucatec Maya at the syntax-processing interface. In A. Machicao y Priemer, A. Nolda, & A. Sioupi (Eds.), Zwischen Kern und Peripherie [Between core and periphery: Studies on peripheral phenomena in language and grammar] (Studia Grammatica 76) (pp. 181–208). Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
  • Butler, L. K., Bohnemeyer, J. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Effects of animacy on sentence production: Ease of retrieval or topicality? Manuscript submitted for publication. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester.
  • Caballero, G. & Kapatsinski, V. (2014). Perceptual functionality of morphological redundancy in Choguita Rarámuri (Tarahumara). Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1134–1143.
  • Carreiras, M., Dunabeitia, J. A., Vergara, M., De La Cruz-Pavia, I. & Laka, I. (2010). Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque. Cognition, 115, 79–92. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.012
  • Casagrande, J. B. (1960). The southwest project in comparative psycholinguistics: A preliminary report. In A. Wallace (Ed.), Men and cultures (selected papers of the fifth international congress of anthropologicl and ethnological sciences, Philadelphia, September 1–9, 1956) (pp. 777–782). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234–272. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234
  • Chen, B., Ning, A., Bi, H., & Dunlap, S. (2008). Chinese subject-relative clauses are more difficult to process than the object-relative clauses. Acta Psychologica, 129, 61–65. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.04.005
  • Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are nouns easier to learn than verbs? Three experimental studies. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170009.003.0013
  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 489–509. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004998
  • Christianson, K., & Ferreira, F. (2005). Conceptual accessibility and sentence production in a free word order language (Odawa). Cognition, 98(2), 105–135. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.006
  • Clemens, L. E., Coon, J., Pedro, P. M., Morgan, A. M., Polinsky, M., Tandet, G., & Wagers, M. (2015). Ergativity and the complexity of extraction: A view from Mayan. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 33(2), 417–467. doi:10.1007/s11049-014-9260-x
  • Cohen, L., & Mehler, J. (1996). Click monitoring revisited: An on-line study of sentence comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 24(1), 94–102. doi:10.3758/BF03197275
  • Costa, A., Alario, F. X., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2007). Cross linguistic research on language production. In M. G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 531–546). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568971.013.0032
  • Crocker, M. (1996). Computational psycholinguistics: An interdisciplinary approach to the study of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-1600-5
  • Cuetos, F., & Mitchell, D. C. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition, 30, 73–105. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90004-2
  • Cutler, A. (1985). Cross-language psycholinguistics. Linguistics, 23, 659–667. doi:10.1515/ling.1985.23.5.659
  • Cutler, A. (2009). Psycholinguistics in our time. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Inside psychology: A science over 50 years (pp. 91–101). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cutler, A., Mehler, H., Norris, D. G., & Segui, J. (1983). A language-specific comprehension strategy. Nature, 304, 159–160. doi:10.1038/304159a0
  • Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385–400. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90033-1
  • Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1989). Limits on bilingualism. Nature, 340, 229–230. doi: 10.1038/340229a0
  • Dediu, D., & Ladd, D. R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(26), 10944–10949. doi:10.1073/pnas.0610848104
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Ehrlich, K., Fernández, E., Fodor, J., Stenshoel, E., & Vinereanu, M. (1999). Low attachment of relative clauses: New data from Swedish, Norwegian and Romanian. Poster presented at the 12th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New York, NY, March 18–20.
  • Elman, J. L., Hare, M., & McRae, K. (2004). Cues, constraints, and competition in sentence processing. In M. Tomasello & D. Slobin (Eds.), Beyond nature-nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates (pp. 111–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429–492. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999094X
  • Fedorova, O., & Yanovich, I. (2004). Relative clause attachment in Russian: The role of constituent length. Talk presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP). Aix-en-Provence, France.
  • Fedorova, O., & Yanovich, I. (2006). Early preferences in RC-attachment in Russian: The effect of working memory differences. In J. Lavine, S. Franks, M. Tasseva-Kurktchieva, & H. Filip (Eds.), Proceedings of FASL 14 (pp. 113–128). Ann Arbor: Michigan Slavic Publications.
  • Fedzechkina, M., Jaeger, T. F., & Newport, E. L. (2012). Language learners restructure their input to facilitate efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(44), 17897–17902. doi:10.1073/pnas.1215776109
  • Fedzechkina, M., Newport, E. L., & Jaeger, T. F. (in press). The miniature artificial language learning paradigm as a complement to typological data. In L. Ortega, A. Tyler, H. I. Park & M. Uno (Eds.), The Usage-based Study of Language Learning and Multilingualism. Georgetown: GUP.
  • Fedzechkina, M., Newport, E. L., & Jaeger, T. F. (resubmitted). Balancing effort and information transmission during language acquisition: Evidence from word order and case-marking. Submitted for consideration to Cognitive Science.
  • Ferreira, F., & Swets, B. (2002). How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(1), 57–84. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2797
  • Fisher, C. (2002). The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: A reply to Tomasello (2000). Cognition, 82, 259–278. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00159-7
  • Fitneva, S., & Matsui, T. (Eds.), (2009). Evidentiality: A window into language and cognitive development. San Francisco, CA: Wiley.
  • Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 203–218. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90156-1
  • Forster, K. I. (1966). Left-to-right processes in the construction of sentences. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 5(3), 285–291. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80032-4
  • Forster, K. I. (1968). Sentence completion in left-and right-branching languages. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7(2), 296–299. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(68)80004-0
  • Forster, K. I., & Clyne, M. G. (1968). Sentence construction in German-English bilinguals. Language and speech, 11(2), 113–119.
  • Fortescue, M., & Lennert Olsen, L. (1992). The acquisition of West Greenlandic. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 111–220).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Foss, D. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1973). On the psychological reality of the phoneme: Perception, identification, and consciousness. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 246–257. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80069-6
  • Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J., & Mehler, J. (1980). Monitoring around the relative clause. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 328–337. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90257-1
  • Frazier, L. (1979). On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies (Ph.D. dissertation). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.
  • Frazier, L. (1985). Syntactic complexity. In D. Dowty, L. Karttunen, & A. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural language parsing (pp. 129–187). Cambridge: University Press.
  • Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5, 519–559. doi:10.1007/BF00138988
  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1996). Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Frazier, L., & Clifton Jr, C. (1997). Construal: Overview, motivation, and some new evidence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(3), 277–295. doi:10.1023/A:1025024524133
  • Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178–210. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(82)90008-1
  • Frenck-Mestre, C., & Pynte, J. (2000). Resolving syntactic ambiguities: Crosslinguistic differences? In M. D. Vincenzi & V. Lombardo (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives on language processing (pp. 119–148). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi:10.1007/978-94-011-3949-6_5
  • Fung, S. S. (2011). The emergence of serial verb constructions in child Cantonese (M. Phil. thesis). Hong Kong: The University of Hong Kong.
  • Futrell, R., Mahowald, K., & Gibson, E. (2015). Large-scale evidence of dependency length minimization in 37 languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(33), 10336–10341. doi:10.1073/pnas.1502134112
  • Futrell, R., Mahowald, K., & Gibson, E. (2015). Quantifying word order freedom in dependency corpora. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Dependency Linguistics, Uppsala, Sweden.
  • Garnham, A. (1994). Future directions. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 1123–1144). San Diego: Academic Press.
  • Gennari, S. P., Mirković, J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2012). Animacy and competition in relative clause production: A cross-linguistic investigation. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 141–176. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.03.002
  • Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Language, cognition, and culture (pp. 301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2009). Early acquisition of nouns and verbs: Evidence from Navajo. In V. C. Mueller Gathercole (Ed.), Routes to language: Studies in honor of Melissa Bowerman (pp. 5–32). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1
  • Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Y. Miyashita, A.P. Marantz, & W. O'Neil (Eds.), Image, Language, Brain, (pp. 95–126), Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Gibson, E., Desmet, T., Grodner, D., Watson, D., & Ko, K. (2005). Reading relative clauses in English. Language & Cognitive Processes, 16(2), 313–353. doi:10.1515/cogl.2005.16.2.313
  • Gibson, E., Pearlmutter, N., Canseco-Gonzalez, E., & Hickok, G. (1996). Recency preference in the human sentence processing mechanism. Cognition, 59, 23–59. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00687-7
  • Gibson, E., & Wu, H.-H. I. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses in context. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(1/2), 125–155. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.536656
  • Gildea, D., & Jaeger, T. F. (submitted). Language structure shaped by the brain: Human languages order information efficiently.
  • Gildea, D., & Temperley, D. (2010). Do grammars minimize dependency length? Cognitive Science, 34(2), 286–310. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01073.x
  • Gleitman, L. R., January, D., Nappa, R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2007). On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544–569. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.007
  • Gopnik, A., & Choi, S. (1995). Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. In M. Tomasello & W. E. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children's acquisition of verbs (pp. 63–80). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1411–1423. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411
  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2004). Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 97–114. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.003
  • Gouvea, A., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2002, March). Relative clause processing and extraposition in Brazilian Portuguese and English. Poster presented at the 15th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing.
  • Graff, P., & Jaeger, T. (2009). Locality and feature specificity in OCP effects: Evidence from Aymara, Dutch, and Javanese. In Proceedings from the annual meeting of the Chicago linguistic society (Vol. 45, No. 1, pp. 127–141). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
  • Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language (pp. 73–113). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274–279. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00255
  • Grillo, N. & Costa, J. (2014). A novel argument for the universality of parsing principles. Cognition, 133(1), 156–187. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.019
  • Grodner, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentential complexity. Cognitive Science, 29(2), 261–290. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_7
  • Gutierrez-Ziardegi, E., Carreiras, M., & Laka, I. (2004). Bilingual sentence processing: Relative clause attachment in Basque and Spanish. Paper presented at the CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, University of Maryland.
  • Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic early parser as a psycholinguistic model. In Proceedings of the second meeting of the North American chapter of the association for computational linguistics (pp. 159–166). doi:10.3115/1073336.1073357
  • Haspelmath, M. (2006). Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics, 42(1), 25–70. doi:10.1017/S0022226705003683
  • Haspelmath, M. (2009). The best-supported language universals refer to scalar patterns deriving from processing cost. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 457–458. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990689
  • Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge University Press.
  • Hawkins, J. A. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199252695.001.0001
  • Hawkins, J. A. (2007). Processing typology and why psychologists need to know about it. New Ideas in Psychology, 25(2), 87–107. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.003
  • Hawkins, J. A. (2014). Cross-linguistic variation and efficiency. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199664993.001.0001
  • Healy, A. F., & Cutting, J. E. (1976). Units of speech perception: Phoneme and syllable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 73–83. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(76)90008-6
  • Hemforth, B., Konieczny, L., Scheepers, C., & Strube, G. (1998). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in German. In D. Hillert (Ed.), Syntax and semantics: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 293–312). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
  • Holmes, V. M., & O'Regan, J. K. (1981). Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 417–430. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90533-8
  • Hsiao, F., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition, 90(11), 3–27. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00124-0
  • von Humboldt, W. (1963). Schriften zur Sprachpfilosophie. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
  • Hwang, H., & Kaiser, E. (2015). Accessibility effects on production vary cross-linguistically: Evidence from English and Korean. Journal of Memory and Language, 84, 190–204. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2015.06.004
  • Imai, M., Haryu, E., & Okada, H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children?. Child development, 76(2), 340–355. j.1749-818X.2010./j.1467-8624.2005.00849_a.x doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849_a.x
  • Imedadze, N., & Tuite, K. (1992). The acquisition of Georgian. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. III, pp. 39–109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Ishizuka, T., Nakatani, K., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing Japanese relative clauses in context. Paper presented at the 19th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing, March 23–25, CUNY Graduate School & University Center, New York, NY.
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2010). Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61(1), 23–62. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.02.002
  • Jaeger, T. F., & Buz, E. (accepted for publication). Signal reduction and linguistic encoding. In E. M. Fernandez & H. S. Cairns (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. Wiley-Black well.
  • Jaeger, T. F., & Norcliffe, E. (2009). The cross-linguistic study of sentence production: State of the art and a call for action. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 866–887. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00147.x
  • Jaeger, T. F., & Tily, H. (2011). On language “utility”: Processing complexity and communicative efficiency. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 323–335. doi:10.1002/wcs.126
  • Jäger, L., Chen, Z., Li, Q., Lin, C-J. C., & Vasishth, S. (2015). The subject relative advantage in Chinese: Evidence for expectation-based processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 79–80, 97–120. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.005
  • Johnston, J. R., & Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. Journal of Child Language, 6, 531–547. doi:10.1017/S030500090000252X
  • Jun, S.-A. (2003). Prosodic phrasing and attachment preferences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 219–249. doi:10.1023/A:1022452408944
  • Kamide, Y., & Mitchell, D. (1997). Relative clause attachment: Nondeterminism in Japanese parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 247–254. doi:10.1023/A:1025017817290
  • Kanno, K., & Nakamura, M. (2001). Processing of relative clauses by Japanese native speakers and L2 learners. Journal of Association for Japanese Language Education, 4, 134–148.
  • Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic inquiry, 8(1), 63–99.
  • Keenan, E. L., & Hawkins, S. (1987). The psychological validity of the accessibility hierarchy. Universal grammar, 15, 60–85.
  • Kenyeres, E. (1927). Les premiers mots de l'entfant et l'apparation des espèces de mots dans son langage. Archives de Psychologie, 20, 191–218.
  • Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An incremental procedural grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive Science, 11, 201–258. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1102_5
  • Kgolo, N., & Eisenbeiss, S. (2015). The role of morphological structure in the processing of complex forms: Evidence from Setswana deverbative nouns. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1116–1133.
  • King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 580–602. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-H
  • King, J. & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 376–395. doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.3.376
  • Konieczny, L. (1996). Human sentence processing: A semantics-oriented approach (Ph.D. dissertation). University of Freiburg, Germany.
  • Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 627–645. doi:10.1023/A:1026528912821
  • Konieczny, L., & Döring, P. (2003). Anticipation of clause-final heads: Evidence from eye-tracking and SRNs. In Proceedings of the ICCS/ASCS-2003 joint international conference on cognitive science. Sydney, Australia 13–17 July 2003.
  • Konieczny, L., & Hemforth, B. (2000). Modifier attachment in German: Relative clauses and prepositional phrases. In A. Kennedy & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 517–527). Amsterdam: Elsevier. doi:10.1016/B978-008043642-5/50024-3
  • Konopka, A. E., & Bock, K. (2009). Lexical or syntactic control of sentence formulation? Structural generalizations from idiom production. Cognitive Psychology, 58(1), 68–101. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.05.002
  • Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Priming sentence planning. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 1–40. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.04.001
  • Kuchinsky, S. E. (2009). From seeing to saying: Perceiving, planning, producing (Ph.D. dissertation). University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois.
  • Kuchinsky, S. E., & Bock, K. (2010, March). From seeing to saying: Perceiving, planning, producing. Paper presented at the 23rd meeting of the CUNY human sentence processing conference, New York, NY.
  • Kuperman, V., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2011). Effects of individual differences in verbal skills on eye-movement patterns during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 42–73. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.03.002
  • Kwon, N. (2008). Processing of syntactic and anaphoric gap-filler dependencies in Korean: Evidence from self-paced reading time, ERP and eye-tracking experiments (Ph.D. dissertation). University of California, San Diego, CA.
  • Kwon, N., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence from ERP data. Language, 89(3), 537–585. doi:10.1353/lan.2013.0044
  • Kwon, N., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence from ERP data. Language, 89(3), 537. doi:10.1353/lan.2013.0044
  • Kwon, N., Lee, Y., Gordon, P. C., Kluender, R., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting subject/object asymmetry: An eye-tracking study of pre-nominal relative clauses in Korean. Language, 86, 546–582. doi:10.1353/lan.2010.0006
  • LaCross, A. (2014). Khalkha Mongolian speakers’ vowel bias: L1 influences on the acquisition of non-adjacent vocalic dependencies. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1033–1047.
  • Lago, S. (2014). Memory and prediction in cross-linguistic sentence comprehension (Ph.D. dissertation). University of Maryland, College Park, MD.
  • Law, D. (2013). Mayan Historical Linguistics in a New Age. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 141–156. doi:10.1111/lnc3.12012
  • Lee, D., & Kweon, S. (2004). A sentence processing study of relative clause in Korean with two attachment sites. Discourse and Cognition, 11, 126–141.
  • Lee, J., & Naigles, L. R. (2005). Input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: A role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 41, 529–540. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.529
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1975). What became of LAD. Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press.
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (2013). A history of psycholinguistics: The pre-chomskyan era. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 1126–1177. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006
  • Levy, R., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2013). The syntactic complexity of Russian relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 461–495. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.10.005
  • Levy, R. & Gibson, E. (2013). Surprisal, the PDC, and the primary locus of processing difficulty in relative clauses. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(229). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00229
  • Levy, R., & Keller, F. (2013). Expectation and locality effects in German verb-final structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 199–222. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.005
  • Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(10), 447–454. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007
  • Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1976). Subject and topic: A new typology of language. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 457–489). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Lin, C., & Bever, T. (2006, March). Chinese is no exception: Universal subject preference of relative clause processing. Paper presented at the 19th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing CUNY Graduate Center, NY.
  • Lin, Y. & Garnsey, S. M. (2007). Plausibility and the resolution of temporary ambiguity in relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. Poster presented at the 20th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing, University of California, San Diego, CA, March 2007.
  • Lipski, J.M. (2014). From “more” to “less”: Spanish, Palenquero (Afro-Colombian creole), and gender agreement. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1144–1155.
  • MacDonald, M. C. (1994). Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 157–201. doi:10.1080/01690969408402115
  • MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(226). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226
  • MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109(1), 35–54. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.35
  • MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676–703. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.676
  • MacDonald, M. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2006). Constraint satisfaction accounts of lexical and sentence comprehension. Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 2, 581–611. doi:10.1016/B978-012369374-7/50016-X
  • MacWhinney, B. (1977). Starting points. Language, 53, 152–168. doi: 10.2307/413059
  • MacWhinney, B. (1987). Applying the competition model to bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8(4), 315–327. doi:10.1017/S0142716400000357
  • MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 49–67). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: A cross-cultural developmental study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(5), 539–558. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90326-2
  • MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (Eds.). (1989). The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 466–490. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.01.001
  • Mazuka, R., & Lust, X. (1990). On parameter setting and parsing: Predictions for cross-linguistic differences in adult and child processing. In L. Frazier & J. de Villiers (Eds.), Language processing and language acquisition (pp. 163–205). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi:10.1007/978-94-011-3808-6_7
  • McDaniel, D., McKee, C., Cowart, W., & Garrett, M. F. (2015). The role of the language production system in shaping grammars. Language, 91(2), 415–441. doi:10.1353/lan.2015.0021
  • McDonald, J. L., & Heilenman, L. K. (1991). Determinants of cue strength in adult first and second language speakers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12(3), 313–348. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009255
  • Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition, 23, 477–494. doi:10.3758/BF03197249
  • Mehler, J., Dommergues, J. Y., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Segui, J. (1981). The syllable's role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 298–305. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90450-3
  • Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Nazzi, T., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (1996). Coping with linguistic diversity: The infant's viewpoint. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 101–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Mehler, J., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Altmann, G., Dupoux, E., Cristophe, A., & Pallier, C. (1993). Understanding compressed sentences: The role of rhythm and meaning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 272–282. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb22975.x
  • Mitchell, D. C., & Brysbaert, M. (1998). Challenges to recent theories of crosslinguistic variation in parsing: Evidence from Dutch. Syntax and semantics: A crosslinguistic perspective, 313–335. doi:10.1108/S0092-4563(1998)0000031019
  • Mitchell, D. C., Cuetos, F., Corley, M. M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (1995). Exposure-based models of human parsing: Evidence for the use of coarse-grained (nonlexical) statistical records. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 469–488. doi:10.1007/BF02143162
  • Miyamoto, E. (1999). Relative clause processing in Brazilian Portuguese and in Japanese (Ph.D. dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, MA.
  • Miyamoto, E. T., & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In G. Garding, & M. Tsujimura (Eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL (Vol. 22, pp. 342–355).
  • Morais, J., Cary, L., Algeria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(79)90020-9
  • Myachykov, A., Garrod, S., & Scheepers, C. (2010). Perceptual priming of structural choice during English and Finnish sentence production. In R. K. Mishra & N. Srinivasan (Eds.), Language & cognition: State of the art (pp. 54–72). Munich: Lincom Europa.
  • Myachykov, A., Thompson, D., Scheepers, C., & Garrod, S. (2011). Visual attention and structural choice in sentence production across languages. Language and Linguistic Compass, 5, 95–107. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00265.x
  • Myachykov, A., & Tomlin, R. S. (2008). Perceptual priming and structural choice in Russian sentence production. Journal of Cognitive Science, 6, 31–48. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/44842550_Perceptual_priming_and_structural_choice_in_Russian_sentence_production
  • Nakatani, K., & Gibson, E. (2008). Distinguishing theories of syntactic expectation cost in sentence comprehension: Evidence from Japanese. Linguistics, 46(1), 63–87. doi:10.1515/LING.2008.003
  • Narasimhan, B. (2005). Splitting the notion of “agent”: Case-marking in early child Hindi. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 787–803. doi:10.1017/S0305000905007117
  • Norcliffe, E., & Konopka, A. E. (2015). Vision and language in cross-linguistic research on sentence production. In R. K. Mishra, N. Srinivasan, & F. Huettig (Eds.), Attention and vision in language processing. New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-81-322-2443-3_5
  • Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Word order affects the time course of sentence formulation in Tzeltal. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1187–1208. doi:10.1080/23273798.2015.1006238
  • Ochs, E. (1982). Ergativity and word order in Samoan child language. Language, 58, 646–671. doi:10.2307/413852
  • O'Grady, W. D. (1997). Syntactic development. University of Chicago Press. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226620787.001.0001
  • Özge, D., Marinis, T., & Zeyrek, D. (2015). Incremental processing in head-final child language: On-line comprehension of relative clauses in Turkish-speaking children and adults. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1230–1243. doi:10.1080/23273798.2014.995108
  • Pae, S. (1993). Early vocabulary in Korean: Are nouns easier to learn than verbs? (Ph.D. dissertation). University of Kansas, Lawrence.
  • Papadopoulou, D., & Clahsen, H. (2003). Parsing strategies in l1 and l2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 501–528. doi:10.1017/s0272263103000214
  • Papafragou, A., Li, P., Choi, Y., & Han, C. (2007). Evidentiality in language and cognition. Cognition, 103, 253–299. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.001
  • Pedersen, H. (1962/1931). The discovery of language: Linguistic science in the nineteenth century. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
  • Pham, H. & Baayen, H. (2015). Vietnamese compounds show an anti-frequency effect in visual lexical decision. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1077–1095.
  • Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2011). Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(9), 3526–3529. doi:10.1073/pnas.1012551108
  • Polinsky, M., Gallo Gómez, C., Graff, P., & Kravtchenko, E. (2012). Subject preference and ergativity. Lingua, 122, 267–277. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2011.11.004
  • Pye, C. (1990). The acquisition of ergative languages. Linguistics, 28, 1291–1330. doi:10.1515/ling.1990.28.6.1291
  • Pye, C. (1992). The acquisition of K'iche’ (Maya). In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 221–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Qiao, X., Shen, L., & Forster, K. (2012). Relative clause processing in Mandarin: Evidence from the maze task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 611–630. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.578394
  • Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2007). Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 1–23. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.014
  • Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman, J. L. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 348–379. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.002
  • Ros, I., Santesteban, M., Fukumura, K. & Laka, I. (2014). Aiming at shorter dependencies: the role of agreement morphology. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1156–1174.
  • Santesteban, M., Pickering, M., Laka, I. & Branigan, H. (2015). Effects of case marking and head position on language production? Evidence from an ergative OV language. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1175–1186.
  • Sanz, M., Laka, I., & Tanenhaus, M. (2013). Sentence comprehension before and after 1970: Topics, debates and techniques. In M. Sanz, I. Laka, & M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Language down the garden path: The cognitive and biological bases for linguistic structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199677139.003.0002
  • Sauppe, S., Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Van Valin Jr, R. D., & Levinson, S. C. (2013). Dependencies first: Eye-tracking evidence from sentence production in Tagalog. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & E. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 1265–1270). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Savin, H. B., & Bever, T. G. (1970). The nonperceptual reality of the phoneme. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 295–302. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80064-0
  • Schriefers, H., Friederici, A. D., & Kuhn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 499–520. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1023
  • Segui, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Mehler, J. (1981). Phoneme monitoring, syllable monitoring and lexical access. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 471–477. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1981.tb01776.x
  • Sekerina, I. (2002). The Late Closure Principle vs. the Balance Principle: Evidence from On-Line Processing of Ambiguous Russian Sentences. In P. Costa & J. Frasek (Eds.), Current Approaches to Formal Slavic Linguistics. Contributions of the Second European Conference on Formal Description of Slavic Languages FDSL II. Potsdam Meeting 1997 (pp. 205–217). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
  • Shen, X. (2006). Late assignment of syntax theory: Evidence from Chinese and English (Ph.D. dissertation), University of Exeter.
  • Sinnemäki, K. (2014). Cognitive processing, language typology, and variation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(4), 477–487. doi:10.1002/wcs.1294
  • Slobin, D. I. (1968). Early grammatical development in several languages, with special attention to Soviet research. Technical Report No. 11. Berkeley: Language-Behavior Research Laboratory, University of California.
  • Slobin, D. I. (1970). Universals of grammatical development in children. In G. Flores D'Arcais & W. Levelt (Eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (pp.174–186). Amsterdam: North Holland.
  • Slobin, D. I. (1977). Language change in childhood and in history. In J. Macnamara (Ed.), Language learning and thought (pp. 185–214). New York: Academic Press.
  • Slobin, D. I. (1985). Why study language crosslinguistically? In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, the data (Vol. 1, pp. 3–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Slobin, D. I. (Ed.) (1985–1997). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vols. 5). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Slobin, D. I. (1992). Introduction. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 1–13). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Slobin, D. I., & Bever, T. G. (1982). Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition, 12, 229–265. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(82)90033-6
  • Sokolov, J. L. (1988). Cue validity in Hebrew sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 15(1), 129–155. doi:10.1017/S0305000900012095
  • Spivey-Knowlton, M., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition, 55, 227–267. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00647-4
  • Spivey-Knowlton, M., Trueswell, J., & Tanenhaus, M. (1993). Context effects in syntactic ambiguity resolution: Discourse and semantic influences in parsing reduced relative clauses. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 276–309. doi:10.1037/h0078826
  • Steinthal, H. (1871/1881). Einleitung in die Psychologie und Sprachwissenschaft. Berlin: F. Dümmler.
  • Stoll, S. (2009). Crosslinguistic approaches to language acquisition. In E. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 89–104). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511576164.006
  • Stoll, S., & Lieven, E. (2014). Studying language acquisition cross-linguistically. In H. Winskel & P. Pradakannaya (Eds.), South and Southeast Asian psycholinguistics (pp. 19–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9781139084642.004
  • Swets, B., Jacovina, M. E., & Gerrig, R. J. (2014). Individual differences in the scope of speech planning: Evidence from eye-movements. Language and Cognition 6, 12–44. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2013.5
  • Taman, H. A. (1993). The utilization of syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic cues in the assignment of subject role in Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14(3), 299–317. doi:10.1017/S014271640001081X
  • Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers’ early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 492–504. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.492
  • Tardif, T., Gelman, S. A., & Xu, F. (1999). Putting the noun bias in context: A comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Development, 70(3), 620–635. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00045
  • Thornton, R., MacDonald, M. C., & Gil, M. (1999). Pragmatic constraint on the interpretation of complex noun phrases in Spanish and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 1347. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.6.1347
  • Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 69–90. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2836
  • Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285–318. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1014
  • Turnbull, R., Burdin, R., Clopper, C., & Tonhauser, J. (2015). Contextual predictability and the prosodic realisation of focus: A cross-linguistic comparison. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1061–1076.
  • Ueno, M., & Garnsey, S. M. (2008). An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Language & Cognitive Processes, 23(5), 646–688. doi:10.1080/01690960701653501
  • Ussishkin, A., Dawson, C., Wedel, A. & Schluter, K. (2015). Auditory masked priming in Maltese spoken word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1096–1115.
  • Van Valin Jr., R. D. (1992). An overview of ergative phenomena and their implications for language acquisition. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 15–37). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Van de Velde, M., Meyer, A. S., & Konopka, A. E. (2014). Message formulation and structural assembly: Describing “easy” and “hard” events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 124–144. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.11.001
  • Vasishth, S., Chen, Z. Li, Q., & Guo, G. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses: Evidence for the subject-relative advantage. PLoS ONE, 8(10), 1–14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077006
  • Vasishth, S., & Drenhaus, H. (2011). Locality in German. Dialogue and Discourse, 2, 59–82. doi:10.5087/dad.2011.104
  • Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. L. (2006). Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and anti-locality effects. Language, 82(4), 767–794. doi:10.1353/lan.2006.0236
  • Wagner, V., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H. (2010). On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. Journal of Experiment Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 423–440. doi:10.1037/a0018619
  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 119–161). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Wasow, T. (1997). End-weight from the speaker's perspective. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research, 26, 347–62. doi:10.1023/A:1025080709112
  • Weinberg, A. (1999). A minimalist theory of human sentence processing. In S. Epstein & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Working minimalism (pp. 282–315). Cambridge: MIT.
  • Wiener, S. & Ito, K. (2014). Do syllable-specific tonal probabilities guide lexical access? Evidence from Mandarin, Shanghai and Cantonese. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1048–1060.
  • Whalen, D. H., & McDonough, J. (2015). Taking the laboratory into the field. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 395–415. doi:10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124915
  • Wundt, W. (1920). Erlebtes und Erkanntes. Leipzig and Stuttgart: Kröner.
  • Yasunaga, D., Yano, M., Yasugi, Y. & Koizumi, M. (2015). Is the subject-before-object preference universal? An ERP study in the Kaqchikel Mayan language. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1209–1229.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.