182
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLES

Spoilt for choice: initially considering several referential expressions affects subsequent referential decisions

, &
Pages 618-632 | Received 21 Apr 2017, Accepted 27 Oct 2017, Published online: 15 Nov 2017

References

  • Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1999). The fan effect: New results and new theories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 186–197. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.2.186
  • Bangerter, A., & Clark, H. H. (2003). Navigating joint projects with dialogue. Cognitive Science, 27, 195–225. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2702_3
  • Bangerter, A., Clark, H. H., & Katz, A. R. (2004). Navigating joint projects in telephone conversations. Discourse Processes, 37, 1–23. doi: 10.1207/s15326950dp3701_1
  • Barr, D. J., Jackson, L., & Phillips, I. (2014). Using a voice to put a name to a face: The psycholinguistics of proper name comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 404–413. doi: 10.1037/a0031813
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1482–1493. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482
  • Brennan, S. E., & Hanna, J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 274–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01019.x
  • Brennan, S. E., & Ohaeri, J. O. (1999). Why do electronic conversations seem less polite? The costs and benefits of hedging. In Proceedings of the international joint conference on work activities coordination and collaboration (pp. 227–235). New York, NY: ACM. doi: 10.1145/295665.295942
  • Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 171–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.003
  • Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). Beyond common and privileged: Gradient representations of common ground in real-time language use. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 62–89. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.543363
  • Bunting, M. F., Conway, A. R. A., & Heitz, R. P. (2004). Individual differences in the fan effect and working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 604–622. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.007
  • Clark, H. H. (1992). Arenas of language use. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Clark, H. H., & Bangerter, A. (2004). Changing conceptions of reference. In I. Noveck & D. Sperber (Eds.), Experimental pragmatics (pp. 25–49). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 127–149). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Clark, H. H., & Krych, M. A. (2004). Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 62–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.08.004
  • Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In A. K. Joshi, B. L. Webber, & I. A. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (Vol. 2, pp. 10–63). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Clark, H. H., & Schaefer, E. F. (1989). Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science, 13, 259–294. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1302_7
  • Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22, 1–39. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90010-7
  • Duff, M. C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2006). Development of shared information in communication despite hippocampal amnesia. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 140–146. doi: 10.1038/nn1601
  • Fox Tree, J. E. (2010). Discourse markers across speakers and settings. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 1–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00195.x
  • Gorman, K. S., Gegg-Harrison, W., Marsh, C. R., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2013). What’s learned together stays together: Speakers’ choice of referring expression reflects shared experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 843–853. doi: 10.1037/a0029467
  • Horton, W. S. (2007). The influence of partner-specific memory associations on language production: Evidence from picture naming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 1114–1139. doi: 10.1080/01690960701402933
  • Horton, W. S. (2008). A memory-based approach to common ground and audience design. In I. Kecskes (Ed.), Intention, common ground, and the egocentric speaker-hearer (pp. 189–222). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Horton, W. S., & Gerrig, R. J. (2005a). Conversational common ground and memory processes in language production. Discourse Processes, 40, 1–35. doi: 10.1207/s15326950dp4001_1
  • Horton, W. S., & Gerrig, R. J. (2005b). The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production. Cognition, 96, 127–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.001
  • Horton, W. S., & Gerrig, R. J. (2016). Revisiting the memory-based processing approach to common ground. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 780–795. doi: 10.1111/tops.12216
  • Horton, W. S., & Slaten, D. G. (2012). Anticipating who will say what: The influence of speaker-specific memory associations on reference resolution. Memory & Cognition, 40, 113–126. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0135-7
  • Hupet, M., & Chantraine, Y. (1992). Changes in repeated references: Collaboration or repetition effects? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 485–496. doi: 10.1007/BF01067526
  • Hupet, M., Chantraine, Y., & Nef, F. (1993). References in conversation between young and old normal adults. Psychology and Aging, 8, 339–346. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.3.339
  • Hupet, M., Seron, X., & Chantraine, Y. (1991). The effects of the codability and discriminability of the referents on the collaborative referring procedure. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 449–462. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02412.x
  • Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversation between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 26–37. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.116.1.26
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
  • Keselman, H. J., Algina, J., Kowalchuk, R. K., & Wolfinger, R. D. (1999). The analysis of repeated measurements: A comparison of mixed-model satterthwaite F tests and a nonpooled adjusted degrees of freedom multivariate test. Communications in Statistics - Theory and Methods, 28, 2967–2999. doi: 10.1080/03610929908832460
  • Kiernan, K., Tao, J., & Gibbs, P. (2012). Tips and strategies for mixed modelling with SAS/STAT procedures. Presented at the 2012 SAS Global Forum, Orlando, FL.
  • Knutsen, D., & Le Bigot, L. (2014). Capturing egocentric biases in reference reuse during collaborative dialogue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1590–1599. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0620-7
  • Knutsen, D., & Le Bigot, L. (2016). Conceptual match as a determinant of reference reuse in dialogue. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000313
  • Knutsen, D., Ros, C., & Le Bigot, L. (2016). Generating references in naturalistic face-to-face and phone-mediated dialog settings. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 796–818. doi: 10.1111/tops.12218
  • Liu, K., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2012). Hedges enhance memory but inhibit retelling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 892–898. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0275-1
  • MacLeod, C. M. (2011). I said, you said: The production effect gets personal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1197–1202. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0168-8
  • McInnes, F., & Attwater, D. (2004). Turn-taking and grounding in spoken telephone number transfers. Speech Communication, 43, 205–223. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2004.04.001
  • Metzing, C., & Brennan, S. E. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 201–213. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00028-7
  • Mills, G. J. (2014). Dialogue in joint activity: Complementarity, convergence and conventionalization. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 158–173. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.006
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000056
  • Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, R. T. (1991). Mental models and the fan effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 940–953. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.940
  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59
  • Rogers, S. L., Fay, N., & Kendal, R. L. (2016). Stick or switch: A selection heuristic predicts when people take the perspective of others or communicate egocentrically. PLOS ONE, 11, e0159570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159570
  • Rossnagel, C. (2000). Cognitive load and perspective-taking: Applying the automatic-controlled distinction to verbal communication. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 429–445. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200005/06)30:3<429::AID-EJSP3>3.0.CO;2-V
  • Satterthwaite, F. E. (1946). An approximate distribution of estimates of variance components. Biometrics Bulletin, 2, 110–114. doi: 10.2307/3002019
  • Shintel, H., & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 260–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01018.x
  • Slamecka, N., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592
  • Tolins, J., Zeamer, C., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2017). Overhearing dialogues and monologues: How does entrainment lead to more comprehensible referring expressions? Discourse Processes. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2017.1279516
  • Tylén, K., Weed, E., Wallentin, M., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Language as a tool for interacting minds. Mind and Language, 25, 3–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01379.x
  • Wilkes-Gibbs, D., & Clark, H. H. (1992). Coordinating beliefs in conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 183–194. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90010-U
  • Yoon, S. O., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2014). Adjusting conceptual pacts in three-party conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 919–937. doi: 10.1037/a0036161

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.