610
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLES

Electrophysiological patterns of visual word recognition in deaf and hearing readers: an ERP mega-study

, & ORCID Icon
Pages 636-650 | Received 04 Apr 2022, Accepted 03 Oct 2022, Published online: 17 Oct 2022

References

  • Andrews, S., & Hersch, J. (2010). Lexical precision in skilled readers: Individual differences in masked neighbor priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(2), 299–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018366
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014
  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Preprint, arXiv:1406.5823.
  • Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Corina, D., Liu, G., & Neville, H. (2000). Visual attention to the periphery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(17), RC93. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-j0001.2000
  • Bélanger, N. N., Baum, S. R., & Mayberry, R. I. (2012). Reading difficulties in adult deaf readers of French: Phonological codes, not guilty!. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(3), 263–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.568555
  • Bélanger, N. N., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner, K. (2013). Orthographic and phonological preview benefits: Parafoveal processing in skilled and less-skilled deaf readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2237–2252. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.780085
  • Bélanger, N. N., & Rayner, K. (2015). What eye movements reveal about deaf readers. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 220–226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414567527
  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
  • Bottari, D., Caclin, A., Giard, M. H., & Pavani, F. (2011). Changes in early cortical visual processing predict enhanced reactivity in deaf individuals. PloS One, 6(9), e25607. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025607
  • Brown, P. M., & Brewer, L. C. (1996). Cognitive processes of deaf and hearing skilled and less skilled readers. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(4), 263–270. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.deafed.a014301
  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(4), 751–774. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2785
  • Cates, D. M., Traxler, M. J., & Corina, D. P. (2021). Predictors of Reading comprehension in deaf and hearing bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 43(1), 81–123. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716421000412
  • Chen, Y., Davis, M. H., Pulvermüller, F., & Hauk, O. (2015). Early visual word processing is flexible: Evidence from spatiotemporal brain dynamics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(9), 1738–1751. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00815
  • Clark, M. D., Hauser, P. C., Miller, P., Kargin, T., Rathmann, C., Guldenoglu, B., Kubus, O., Spurgeon, E., & Israel, E. (2016). The importance of early sign language acquisition for deaf readers. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 32(2), 127–151. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2013.878123
  • Costello, B., Caffarra, S., Fariña, N., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2021). Reading without phonology: ERP evidence from skilled deaf readers of Spanish. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84490-5
  • Cripps, J. H., McBride, K. A., & Forster, K. I. (2005). Lexical processing with deaf and hearing: Phonology and orthographic masked priming. Journal of Second Language Acquisition and Teaching, 12, 31–44. https://journals.uair.arizona.edu/index.php/AZSLAT/article/view/21285/20864
  • Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004
  • Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open-source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
  • Dufau, S., Grainger, J., Midgley, K. J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2015). A thousand words are worth a picture: Snapshots of printed-word processing in an event-related potential megastudy. Psychological Science, 26(12), 1887–1897. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615603934
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Pearson Assessments.
  • Emmorey, K., & Lee, B. (2021). The neurocognitive basis of skilled Reading in prelingually and profoundly deaf adults. Language and Linguistics Compass, 15(2), e12407. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12407
  • Emmorey, K., Midgley, K. J., Kohen, C. B., Sehyr, Z. S., & Holcomb, P. J. (2017). The N170 ERP component differs in laterality, distribution, and association with continuous Reading measures for deaf and hearing readers. Neuropsychologia, 106, 298–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.001
  • Emmorey, K., Winsler, K., Midgley, K. J., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2020). Neurophysiological correlates of frequency, concreteness, and iconicity in American sign language. Neurobiology of Language, 1(2), 249–267. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00012
  • Fariña, N., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2017). Phonological and orthographic coding in deaf skilled readers. Cognition, 168, 27–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.015
  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 71–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.71
  • Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2009). Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 128–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00121.x
  • Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Vergara-Martínez, M., & Perea, M. (2017). Early use of phonological codes in deaf readers: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 106, 261–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.006
  • Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Vergara-Martínez, M., & Perea, M. (2022). The impact of visual cues during visual word recognition in deaf readers: An ERP study. Cognition, 218, 104938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104938
  • Hauk, O., Davis, M. H., Ford, M., Pulvermüller, F., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2006). The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage, 30(4), 1383–1400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048
  • Hirshorn, E. A., Dye, M. W. G., Hauser, P., Supalla, T. R., & Bavelier, D. (2015). The contribution of phonological knowledge, memory, and language background to reading comprehension in deaf populations. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01153
  • Holcomb, P. J., Grainger, J., & O'rourke, T. (2002). An electrophysiological study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on printed word perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(6), 938–950. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760191153
  • Kiyonaga, K., Grainger, J., Midgley, K., & Holcomb, P. J. (2007). Masked cross-modal repetition priming: An event-related potential investigation. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(3), 337–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600652471
  • Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 804. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.804
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). Lmertest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • Lederberg, A. R., Branum-Martin, L., Webb, M., Schick, B., Antia, S., Easterbrooks, S. R., & Connor, C. M. (2019). Modality and interrelations Among language, Reading, spoken phonological awareness, and fingerspelling. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 24(4), 408–423. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enz011
  • Li, D., Gao, K., Wu, X., Chen, X., Zhang, X., Li, L., & He, W. (2013). Deaf and hard of hearing adolescents’ processing of pictures and written words for taxonomic categories in a priming task of semantic categorization. American Annals of the Deaf, 158(4), 426–437. https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.2013.0040
  • Markwardt, F. C. (1989). Peabody individual achievement test-revised: PIAT-R. American Guidance Service Circle Pines.
  • Mayberry, R. I., del Giudice, A. A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 164–188. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enq049
  • McQuarrie, L., & Parrila, R. (2009). Phonological representations in deaf children: Rethinking the “functional equivalence” hypothesis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(2), 137–154. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enn025
  • Meade, G., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2019). Task modulates ERP effects of orthographic neighborhood for pseudowords but not words. Neuropsychologia, 129, 385–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.014
  • Morford, J. P., Occhino-Kehoe, C., Piñar, P., Wilkinson, E., & Kroll, J. F. (2017). The time course of cross-language activation in deaf ASL–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(2), 337–350. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891500067X
  • Pelli, D. G., Burns, C. W., Farell, B., & Moore-Page, D. C. (2006). Feature detection and letter identification. Vision Research, 46(28), 4646–4674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.04.023
  • Rastle, K. (2019). The place of morphology in learning to read in English. Cortex, 116, 45–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.008
  • Sarchet, T., Marschark, M., Borgna, G., Convertino, C., & Dirmyer, R. (2014). Vocabulary knowledge of deaf and hearing postsecondary students. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 27(2), 161–176. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4280560/
  • Sehyr, Z., & Emmorey, K. (2021). Assessing the contribution of lexical quality and sign language variables to reading comprehension in deaf adult ASL signers. Paper presented at the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, July, Virtual meeting.
  • Strijkers, K., Bertrand, D., & Grainger, J. (2015). Seeing the same words differently: The time course of automaticity and topdown intention in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(8), 1542–1551. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00797
  • Van Heuven, W. J., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.850521
  • Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequencyinevent-related brainpotentials. Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 380–393. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197127
  • Varga, V., Tóth, D., & Csépe, V. (2021). Lexical competition without phonology: Masked orthographic neighbor priming with deaf readers. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 27(2), 151–165. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enab040
  • Villwock, A., Wilkinson, E., Piñar, P., & Morford, J. P. (2021). Language development in deaf bilinguals: Deaf middle school students co-activate written English and American sign language during lexical processing. Cognition, 211, 104642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104642
  • West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1024–1037. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290051137558
  • Winsler, K., Midgley, K. J., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2018). An electrophysiological megastudy of spoken word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(8), 1063–1082. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1455985
  • Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 971–979. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.5.971

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.