1,739
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

A common neural mechanism for speech perception and movement initiation specialized for place of articulation

| (Reviewing Editor)
Article: 1233649 | Received 12 Apr 2016, Accepted 04 Sep 2016, Published online: 26 Sep 2016

References

  • Arsenault, J. S., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2016). No evidence of somatotopic place of articulation feature mapping in motor cortex during passive speech perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1231–1240. doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0988-z
  • Baum, S. R. (2002). Consonant and vowel discrimination by brain-damaged individuals: Effects of phonological segmentation. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 447–461. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00020-8
  • Blumstein, S. E., Cooper, W. E., Zurif, E. B., & Caramazza, A. (1977). The perception and production of voice-onset time in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 15, 371–383. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(77)90089-6
  • Boemio, A., Fromm, S., Braun, A., & Poeppel, D. (2005). Hierarchical and asymmetric temporal sensitivity in human auditory cortices. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 389–395. doi:10.1038/nn1409
  • Bradshaw, J. L., & Nettleton, N. C. (1981). The nature of hemispheric specialization in man. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 51–91. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00007548
  • Bryden, M. P. (1986). Dichotic listening performance, cognitive ability, and cerebral organization. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 445–456. doi:10.1037/h0080108
  • Casini, L., Burle, B., & Nguyen, N. (2009). Speech perception engages a general timer: Evidence from a divided attention word identification task. Cognition, 112, 318–322. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.005
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • D’Ausilio, A., Craighero, L., & Fadiga, L. (2012). The contribution of the frontal lobe to the perception of speech. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 25, 328–335. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.02.003
  • Devlin, J., & Aydelott, J. (2009). Speech perception: Motoric contributions versus the motor theory. Current Biology, 19, R198–R200. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.005
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fitzgerald, L., McKelvey, J., & Szeligo, F. (2002). Mechanisms of dressing apraxia: A case study. Neuropsychiatry, neuropsychology, and behavioral neurology, 15, 148–155.
  • Fridriksson, J., Moss, J., Davis, B., Baylis, G., Bonilha, L., & Rorden, C. (2008). Motor speech perception modulates the cortical language areas. Neuroimage, 41, 605–613. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.046
  • Geffen, G., & Traub, E. (1979). Preferred hand and familial sinistrality in dichotic monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 17, 527–531. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(79)90061-7
  • Heilman, K., Rothi, L., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346. doi:10.1212/WNL.32.4.342
  • Hiscock, M., Inch, R., & Ewing, C. (2005). Constant and variable aspects of the dichotic listening right-ear advantage: A comparison of standard and signal detection tasks. Laterality, 10, 517–534. doi:10.1080/13576500442000283
  • Hugdahl, K., Bodner, T., Weiss, E., & Benke, T. (2003). Dichotic listening performance and frontal lobe function. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 58–65. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00210-0
  • Jamison, H. L., Watkins, K. E., Bishop, D. V., & Matthews, P. (2006). Hemispheric specialization for processing auditory nonspeech stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1266–1275. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj068
  • Keane, A. M. (1999). Cerebral organization of motor programming and verbal processing as a function of degree of hand preference and familial sinistrality. Brain and Cognition, 40, 500–515. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1115
  • Keane, A. M. (2001). Motor control of the hands: The effect of familial sinistrality. International Journal of Neuroscience, 110, 25–41. doi:10.3109/00207450108994219
  • Keane, A. M. (2002). Direction of hand preference: The connection with speech and the influence of familial handedness. International Journal of Neuroscience, 112, 1287–1303. doi:10.1080/00207450290158205
  • Keane, A. M. (2008). What aspect of handedness is general motor programming related to? International Journal of Neuroscience, 118, 519–530. doi:10.1080/00207450601074667
  • Keane, A. M. (2012). The general motor programmer: Its specialization for speech perception and movement. World Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 156–158. doi:10.4236/wjns.2012.23024
  • Khedr, E., Hamed, E., Said, A., & Basahi, J. (2002). Handedness and language cerebral lateralization. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 87, 469–473. doi:10.1007/s00421-002-0652-y
  • Kimura, D. (1977). Acquisition of a motor skill after left-hemisphere damage. Brain, 100, 527–542. doi:10.1093/brain/100.3.527
  • Kimura, D. (1982). Left-hemisphere control of oral and brachial movements and their relation to communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 298, 135–149. doi:10.1098/rstb.1982.0077
  • Kimura, D., & Archibald, Y. (1974). Motor functions of the left hemisphere. Brain, 97, 337–350. doi:10.1093/brain/97.1.337
  • Laguitton, V., De Graaf, J., Chauvel, P., & Liégeois-Chauvel, C. (2000). Identification reaction times of voiced/voiceless continua: A right-ear advantage for VOT values near the phonetic boundary. Brain and Language, 75, 153–162. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2350
  • Lashley, K. S. (1937). Functional determinants of cerebral localization. Archives of neurology and psychiatry, 21, 371–387. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260200143013
  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–146). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Liberman, A. M. (1982). On finding that speech is special. American Psychologist, 37, 148–167. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.148
  • Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90021-6
  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461. doi:10.1037/h0020279
  • Liepmann, H. (1980). The left hemisphere and action (A translation from Munchener medizinische Wochenschrift 1905, 48 & 49). In Translations from Liepmann’s essays on apraxia (Research Bulletin) (506, pp. 17–50). Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario.
  • Lomas, J., & Kimura, D. (1976). Intrahemispheric interaction between speaking and sequential manual activity. Neuropsychologia, 14, 23–33. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(76)90004-X
  • Long, M. A., Katlowitz, K. A., Svirsky, M. A., Clary, R. C., Byun, T. M., Majaj, N., ... Greenlee, J. D. (2016). Functional segregation of cortical regions underlying speech timing and articulation. Neuron, 89, 1187–1193. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.032
  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-4754-8
  • Marstaller, L., & Burianova, H. (2014). The multisensory perception of co-speech gestures—A review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 30, 69–77. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.04.003
  • Mateer, C., & Kimura, D. (1977). Impairment of nonverbal oral movements in aphasia. Brain and Language, 4, 262–276. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(77)90022-0
  • McFarland, K., Ashton, R., & Jeffery, C. K. (1989). Lateralized dual-task performance: The effects of spatial and muscular repositioning. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1267–1276. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90039-0
  • Meador, K., Loring, D., Lee, K., Hughes, M., Lee, G., Nichols, M., & Heilman, K. (1999). Cerebral lateralization: Relationship of language and ideomotor praxis. Neurology, 53, 2028–2031. doi:10.1212/WNL.53.9.2028
  • Moineau, S., Dronkers, N., & Bates, E. (2005). Exploring the processing continuum of single-word comprehension in aphasia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 884–896. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2005/061)
  • Morais, J., & Darwin, C. J. (1974). Ear differences for same-different reaction times to monaurally presented speech. Brain and Language, 1, 383–390. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(74)90015-7
  • Moscovitch, M. (1973). Language and the cerebral hemispheres: Reaction-time studies and their implications for models of cerebral dominance. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Alloway (Eds.), Communication and affect: Language and thought (pp. 89–126). New York, NY: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-558250-6.50012-2
  • Ojemann, G. A. (1984). Common cortical and thalamic mechanisms for language and motor functions. American Journal of Physiology, 246, R901–R903.
  • Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Oscar-Berman, M., Zurif, E. B., & Blumstein, S. (1975). Effects of unilateral brain damage on the processing of speech sounds. Brain and Language, 2, 345–355. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(75)80075-7
  • Perecman, E., & Kellar, L. (1981). The effect of voice and place among aphasic, nonaphasic right-damaged, and normal subjects on a metalinguistic task. Brain and Language, 12, 213–223. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(81)90015-8
  • Perkell, J. (2012). Movement goals and feedback and feedforward control mechanisms in speech production. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 25, 382–407. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.02.011
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329–392. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12001495
  • Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R., ... Eustache, F. (1997). The structural components of music perception: A functional anatomical study. Brain, 120, 229–243. doi:10.1093/brain/120.2.229
  • Pulvermuller, F., Huss, M., Kherif, F., Martin, F., Hauk, O., & Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 7865–7870. doi:10.1073/pnas.0509989103
  • Rastatter, M. P., & Gallaher, A. J. (1982). Reaction-times of normal subjects to monaurally presented verbal and tonal stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 20, 465–473. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(82)90045-8
  • Roy, E., & Square, P. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). North-Holland: Elsevier Science.
  • Russell, D. G. (1976). Spatial location cues and movement production. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Motor control (pp. 67–85). New York, NY: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-665950-4.50008-0
  • Schirmer, A. (2004). Timing speech: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 269–287. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.003
  • Schwartz, J., & Tallal, P. (1980). Rate of acoustic change may underlie hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Science, 207, 1380–1381. doi:10.1126/science.7355297
  • Scott, S. K., & McGettigan, C. (2013). Do temporal processes underlie left hemisphere dominance in speech perception? Brain and Language, 127, 36–45. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.006
  • Segalowitz, S. J., & Cohen, H. (1989). Right hemisphere EEG sensitivity to speech. Brain and Language, 37, 220–231. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(89)90016-3
  • Sheehan, E. P., & Smith, H. V. (1986). Cerebral lateralization and handedness and their effects on verbal and spatial reasoning. Neuropsychologia, 24, 531–540. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(86)90097-7
  • Simos, P. G., Molfese, D. L., & Brenden, R. A. (1997). Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of voicing-cue discrimination: Laterality patterns and development. Brain and Language, 57, 122–150. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1836
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Shankweiler, D. (1970). Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 579–594. doi:10.1121/1.1912174
  • Szenkovits, G., Peelle, J., Norris, D., & Davis, M. (2012). Individual differences in premotor and motor recruitment during speech perception. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1380–1392. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.023
  • Tallal, P. (1983). A precise timing mechanism may underlie a common speech perception and production area in the peri-Sylvian cortex of the dominant hemisphere. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 219–220. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00015636
  • Tallal, P., & Newcombe, F. (1978). Impairment of auditory perception and language comprehension in dysphasia. Brain and Language, 5, 13–24. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(78)90003-2
  • Voyer, D., & Techentin, C. (2009). Dichotic listening with consonant-vowel pairs: The role of place of articulation and stimulus dominance. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 162–172. doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2008.12.001
  • Wallwork, J. F. (1985). Language and linguistics (2nd ed.). Oxford: Heinemann.
  • Watson, N. V., & Kimura, D. (1989). Right-hand superiority for throwing but not for intercepting. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1399–1414. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90133-4
  • Westerhausen, R., Kompus, K., & Hugdahl, K. (2014). Mapping hemispheric symmetries, relative asymmetries, and absolute asymmetries underlying the auditory laterality effect. Neuroimage, 84, 962–970. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.074
  • Wilson, S., Saygin, A., Sereno, M., & Iacoboni, M. (2004). Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 701–702. doi:10.1038/nn1263
  • Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., Ovrut, M., & Drake, C. (1990). Rate and timing precision of motor coordination in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 349–359. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.349