344
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The relationship between visual confirmation bias, belief consistency, and belief polarization

ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 1-38 | Received 30 Jun 2020, Accepted 23 Dec 2021, Published online: 10 May 2022

References

  • Achtziger, A., Alós-Ferrer, C., Hügelschäfer, S., & Steinhauser, M. (2014). The neural basis of belief updating and rational decision making. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 55–62. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss099
  • Ahlstrom, C., Victor, T., Wege, C., & Steinmetz, E. (2011). Processing of eye/head-tracking data in large-scale naturalistic driving data sets. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems, 13(2), 553–564. https://doi.org/10.1109/TITS.2011.2174786
  • Anwar, S., Bayer, P., & Hjalmarsson, R. (2012). The impact of jury race in criminal trials. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127(2), 1017–1055. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjs014
  • Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (2011). Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge University Press.
  • Balcetis, E. (2008). Where the motivation resides and self-deception hides: How motivated cognition accomplishes self-deception. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 361–381. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00042.x
  • Basu, Y., Yan, H., & Ford, D. (2014, November 25). Fires, chaos erupt in Ferguson after grand jury doesn’t indict in Michael Brown case. CNN. www.cnn.com/2014/11/24/justice/ferguson-grand-jury/index.html
  • Batson, C. D. (1975). Rational processing or rationalization? The effect of disconfirming information on a stated religious belief. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(1), 176–184. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076771
  • Becker, G. S. (1976). The economic approach to human behavior. University of Chicago Press.
  • Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 134(2), 207–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207
  • Bock, M. A., & Schneider, D. A. (2017). The voice of lived experience: Mobile video narratives in the courtroom. Information, Communication & Society, 20(3), 335–350. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2016.1168474
  • Boudreau, C., MacKenzie, S. A., & Simmons, D. J. (2019). Police violence and public perceptions: An experimental study of how information and endorsements affect support for law enforcement. The Journal of Politics, 81(3), 1101–1110. https://doi.org/10.1086/703540
  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press. https://doi.org/10.1037/10037-000
  • Brown, M. K., Carnoy, M., Currie, E., Duster, T., & Oppenheimer, D. B. (2003). Whitewashing race: The myth of a color-blind society. University of California Press.
  • Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.72
  • Carney, D. R., Colvin, C. R., & Hall, J. A. (2007). A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), 1054–1072. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.01.004
  • Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012
  • Cesario, J. (2021). What can experimental studies of bias tell us about real-world group disparities? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, 1–80. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X21000017
  • Charman, S. D., Gregory, A. H., & Carlucci, M. (2009). Exploring the diagnostic utility of facial composites: Beliefs of guilt can bias perceived similarity between composite and suspect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 15(1), 76–90. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014682
  • Charman, S. D., Kavetski, M., & Mueller, D. H. (2017). Cognitive bias in the legal system: Police officers evaluate ambiguous evidence in a belief-consistent manner. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(2), 193–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.02.001
  • Dan, E. L., Dînşoreanu, M., & Mureşan, R. C. (2020, May). Accuracy of six interpolation methods applied on pupil diameter data. In 2020 IEEE International Conference on Automation, Quality and Testing, Robotics (AQTR) (pp. 1–5). Cluj-Napoca, Romania: IEEE.
  • De Graef, P., Christiaens, D., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1990). Perceptual effects of scene context on object identification. Psychological Research, 52(4), 317–329. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00868064
  • Dosher, B. A., & Sperling, G. (1998). A century of human information-processing theory: Vision, attention, and memory. In J. Hochberg (Ed.). Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd, pp. 199–252). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012301160-2/50010-1
  • Dunning, D. (2012). Judgment and decision making. In S. T. Fiske & C. N. Macrae (Eds.), The Sage Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 251–272). SAGE Publications Ltd. https://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446247631.n13.
  • Eidelman, S., & Biernat, M. (2003). Derogating black sheep: Individual or group protection? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 602–609. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00042-8
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fink, A. (2010). Survey research methods. In P. Peterson, E. Baker, & B. McGaw (Eds.), International encyclopedia of education (third edition) (pp. 152–160). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-044894-7.00296-7
  • Feigenson, N., & Spiesel, C. (2009). Law on display. New York University Press.
  • Fitzmaurice, G. M., Laird, N. M., & Ware, J. H. (2011). Applied longitudinal analysis: 2nd. In edition. John Wiley & Sons: Wiley.
  • Frankenberg, R. (2001). The mirage of an unmarked whiteness. In B. B. Rasmussen, M. Klinenberg, I. J. Nexica, & M. Wray (Eds.), The making and unmaking of whiteness (pp. 72–96). Duke University Press.
  • Frutos-Pascual, M., & Garcia-Zapirain, B. (2015). Assessing visual attention using eye-tracking sensors in intelligent cognitive therapies based on serious games. Sensors, 15(5), 11092–11117. https://doi.org/10.3390/s150511092
  • Fryer, R. G., Jr, Harms, P., & Jackson, M. O. (2019). Updating beliefs when evidence is open to interpretation: Implications for bias and polarization. Journal of the European Economic Association, 17(5), 1470–1501. https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvy025
  • Garon, M., Lavallee, M. M., Estay, E. V., Beauchamp, M. H., & von Hecker, U. (2018). Visual encoding of social cues predicts sociomoral reasoning. PloS one, 13(7), 93–104. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201099
  • Granot, Y., Balcetis, E., Feigenson, N., & Tyler, T. (2018). In the eyes of the law: Perception versus reality in appraisals of video evidence. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 24(1), 93–104. https://doi.org/10.1037/law0000137
  • Granot, Y., Balcetis, E., Schneider, K. E., & Tyler, T. R. (2014). Justice is not blind: Visual attention exaggerates effects of group identification on legal punishment. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 143(6), 2196–2208. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037893
  • Gibson, J. J. (1933). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074626
  • Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis: International version. Pearson.
  • Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A., Jr, & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.210
  • Holyoak, K. J., & Simon, D. (1999). Bidirectional reasoning in decision making by constraint satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 128(1), 3–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.3
  • Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291. https://doi.org/10.1038/72999
  • Howard, I. P., & Templeton, W. B. (1966). Human spatial orientation. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy: Mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychological Science, 19(9), 848–853. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02167.x
  • Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2(3), 194–203. https://doi.org/10.1038/35058500
  • Jarodzka, H., Holmqvist, K., & Nyström, M. (2010, March). A vector-based, multidimensional scanpath similarity measure. In Proceedings of the 2010 symposium on eye-tracking research & applications (pp. 211–218). New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery.
  • Jin, S., Qing, C., Xu, X., & Wang, Y. (2019, July). Emotion recognition using eye gaze based on shallow CNN with identity mapping. In International Conference on Brain Inspired Cognitive Systems (pp. 65–75). Springer, Cham.
  • John, E. R., Bartlett, F., Shimokochi, M., & Kleinman, D. (1973). Neural readout from memory. Journal of Neurophysiology, 36(5), 893–924. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1973.36.5.893
  • Johnson, M. H. (1990). Cortical maturation and perceptual development. In H. Bloch & B. I. Bertenthal (Eds.), Sensory-motor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 145–162). Springer Science & Business Media.
  • Jones, J. M. (2020, June 22). In U.S., negative emotions surged, then declined in June. Gallup. https://news.gallup.com/poll/312872/negative-emotions-surged-declined-june.aspx
  • Kassin, S. M., Dror, I. E., & Kukucka, J. (2013). The forensic confirmation bias: Problems, perspectives, and proposed solutions. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2(1), 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2013.01.001
  • Kawakami, K., Friesen, J., & Vingilis-Jaremko, L. (2018). Visual attention to members of own and other groups: Preferences, determinants, and consequences. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(4), e12380. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12380
  • Klein, R. M., & Posner, M. I. (1974). Attention to visual and kinesthetic components of skills. Brain Research, 71(2–3), 401–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(74)90984-6
  • Knowles, E. D., Lowery, B. S., Chow, R. M., & Unzueta, M. M. (2014). Deny, distance, or dismantle? How white Americans manage a privileged identity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(6), 594–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614554658
  • Krabbe, P. F. M. (2017). Chapter 7—Validity. In P. F. M. Krabbe (Ed.), The measurement of health and health status (pp. 113–134). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801504-9.00007-6
  • Kübler, T. C., Kasneci, E., & Rosenstiel, W. (2014, March). Subsmatch: Scanpath similarity in dynamic scenes based on subsequence frequencies. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp. 319–322). New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery.
  • Kukucka, J., & Kassin, S. M. (2014). Do confessions taint perceptions of handwriting evidence? An empirical test of the forensic confirmation bias. Law and Human Behavior, 38(3), 256–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000066
  • Lachter, J., Forster, K. I., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Forty-five years after broadbent (1958): Still no identification without attention. Psychological Review, 111(4), 880–913. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.880
  • Land, M. F. (2006). Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 25(3), 296–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.preteyeres.2006.01.002
  • Lane, S. P., & Hennes, E. P. (2019). Conducting sensitivity analyses to identify and buffer power vulnerabilities in studies examining substance use over time. Addictive Behaviors, 94, 117–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.09.017
  • Laubrock, J., Cajar, A., & Engbert, R. (2013). Control of fixation duration during scene viewing by interaction of foveal and peripheral processing. Journal of Vision, 13(12), 11, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.12.11
  • Liberman, A., & Chaiken, S. (1992). Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(6), 669–679. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292186002
  • Lipsitz, G. (1998). The possessive investment in whiteness: How white people profit from identity politics. Temple University Press.
  • Loftus, G. R., & Mackworth, N. H. (1978). Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 4(4), 565–572. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.4.4.565
  • Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2098–2109. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–3514.37.11.2098
  • Marston, L. (2010). Introductory statistics for health and nursing using SPSS. Sage Publications.
  • Mathôt, S., Cristino, F., Gilchrist, I. D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). A simple way to estimate similarity between pairs of eye movement sequences. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.5.1.4
  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Visual attention (R. D. Wright, Ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Maxson, C. L., Hennigan, K., & Sloane, D. C. (2003). Factors that influence public opinion of the police. US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of Justice.
  • Mazzella, R., & Feingold, A. (1994). The effects of physical attractiveness, race, socioeconomic status, and gender of defendants and victims on judgments of Mock Jurors: A meta-analysis 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(15), 1315–1338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01552.x
  • McDermott, M., & Samson, F. L. (2005). White racial and ethnic identity in the United States. Annual Review of Sociology, 31(1), 245–261. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.31.041304.122322
  • McGowen, R., & King, G. D. (1982). Effects of authoritarian, anti-authoritarian, and egalitarian legal attitudes on mock juror and jury decisions. Psychological Reports, 51(3_suppl), 1067–1074. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1982.51.3f.1067
  • McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588), 746–748. https://doi.org/10.1038/264746a0
  • McHugh, M. L. (2012). Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia medica, 22(3), 276–282. https://doi.org/10.11613/BM.2012.031
  • Mnookin, J. L. (1998). The image of truth: Photographic evidence and the power of analogy. The Yale Journal of Law & the Humanities, 10(1), 1–74.
  • Mogg, K., Millar, N., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening facial expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(4), 695–704. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.695
  • Moradi, Z., Najlerahim, A., Macrae, C. N., & Humphreys, G. W. (2020). Attentional saliency and ingroup biases: From society to the brain. Social Neuroscience, 15(3), 324–333. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2020.1716070
  • Morin, R., Parker, K., Stepler, R., & Mercer, A. (2017). Behind the badge. Pew Research Center, 11. https://www.pewresearch.org/social-trends/2017/01/11/behind-the-badge/
  • Mozaffari, S., Klein, P., Viiri, J., Ahmed, S., Kuhn, J., & Dengel, A. (2018, June). Evaluating similarity measures for gaze patterns in the context of representational competence in physics education. In Proceedings of the 2018 ACM Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications (pp. 1–5). New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery.
  • Mullinix, K. J., Bolsen, T., & Norris, R. J. (2021). The feedback effects of controversial police use of force. Political Behavior, 43(2), 881–898. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09646-x
  • Müller, H. J., & Krummenacher, J. (2006). Locus of dimension weighting: Preattentive or postselective? Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 490–513. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280500194154
  • Munro, G. D., & Ditto, P. H. (1997). Biased assimilation, attitude polarization, and affect in reactions to stereotype-relevant scientific information. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(6), 636–653. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297236007
  • New York State Law. (2019). Retrieved June 24, 2020, from https://www.nysenate.gov/legislation/laws/PEN
  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
  • O’Brien, B. (2009). Prime suspect: An examination of factors that aggravate and counteract confirmation bias in criminal investigations. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 15(4), 315–334. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017881
  • Office of the Revisor of Statutes. (2019). Retrieved June 24, 2020, from https://www.revisor.mn.gov/statutes/cite/609.43
  • Pereira, A., Harris, E., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2021). Identity concerns drive belief: The impact of partisan identity on the belief and dissemination of true and false news. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, (pp. 1–24). https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302211030004
  • Pick, H. L., Warren, D. H., & Hay, J. C. (1969). Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction. Perception & Psychophysics, 6(4), 203–205. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03207017
  • Pinto, I. R., Marques, J. M., Levine, J. M., & Abrams, D. (2010). Membership status and subjective group dynamics: Who triggers the black sheep effect? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 107–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018187
  • Plous, S. (1991). Biases in the assimilation of technological breakdowns: Do accidents make us safer? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21(13), 1058–1082. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1991.tb00459.x
  • Posner, M. I. (1967). Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(1), 103–107. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024911
  • Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157
  • Qu-Lee, J., & Balcetis, E. (2020). Accuracy and error: The call for ecological validity is right but missing perceptual idiosyncrasies is wrong. [peer commentary on “what can experimental studies of bias tell us about real-world group disparities?” by Cesario, J]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, 51–53. h ttps://d oi.o rg/1 0.1017/S0140525X21000807, e0
  • Rajsic, J., Ouslis, N. E., Wilson, D. E., & Pratt, J. (2017). Looking sharp: Becoming a search template boosts precision and stability in visual working memory. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79(6), 1643–1651. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1342-5
  • Rajsic, J., Wilson, D. E., & Pratt, J. (2015). Confirmation bias in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 41(5), 1353–1364. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000090
  • Rajsic, J., Wilson, D. E., & Pratt, J. (2018). The price of information: Increased inspection costs reduce the confirmation bias in visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 832–849. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1278249
  • Ramanauskas, N., Daunys, G., & Dervinis, D. (2008, July). Investigation of calibration techniques in video based eye tracking system. In International Conference on Computers for Handicapped Persons (pp. 1208–1215). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
  • Rayner, K., & Morrison, R. E. (1981). Eye movements and identifying words in parafoveal vision. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 17(3), 135–138. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03333690
  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1992). Eye movements and scene perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 46(3), 342–376. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084328
  • Rayner, K., Smith, T. J., Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Eye movements and visual encoding during scene perception. Psychological Science, 20(1), 6–10. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02243.x
  • Rock, I., & Victor, J. (1964). Vision and touch: An experimentally created conflict between the two senses. Science, 143(3606), 594–596. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.143.3606.594
  • Rock, I., & Gutman, D. (1981). The effect of inattention on form perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(2), 275–285. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.2.275
  • Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In T. Brown, E. Reed, & E. Turiel (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. 103, 135). Erlbaum.
  • Rullo, M., Presaghi, F., Livi, S., & McCormick, C. (2015). Reactions to ingroup and outgroup deviants: An experimental group paradigm for black sheep effect. PloS one, 10(5), e0125605. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125605
  • Ryu, D., Cooke, A., Bellomo, E., & Woodman, T. (2020). Watch out for the hazard! Blurring peripheral vision facilitates hazard perception in driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 146, 105755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105755
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
  • Sieswerda, S., Arntz, A., Mertens, I., & Vertommen, S. (2007). Hypervigilance in patients with borderline personality disorder: Specificity, automaticity, and predictors. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(5), 1011–1024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.07.012
  • Sirois, S., & Brisson, J. (2014). Pupillometry. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science, 5(6), 679–692. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1323
  • Süssenbach, P., Bohner, G., & Eyssel, F. (2012). Schematic influences of rape myth acceptance on visual information processing: An eye-tracking approach. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 660–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.005
  • Taber, C. S., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31(2), 137–155. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9075-8
  • Tagg, J. (2009). The disciplinary frame: Photographic truths and the capture of meaning. University of Minnesota Press.
  • Tobii Pro Learning Center. (2015, September 16). How to position the participant and the eye tracker. https://www.tobiipro.com/learn-and-support/learn/steps-in-an-eye-tracking-study/run/how-to-position-the-participant-and-the-eye-tracker/
  • Tsotsos, J. K. (1990). Analyzing vision at the complexity level. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(3), 423–445. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00079577
  • Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2003). The group engagement model: Procedural justice, social identity, and cooperative behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(4), 349–361. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_07
  • Ungerleider, S. K. A. L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 315–341. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
  • van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Urry, H. L., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. Neuroimage, 36(3), 1041–1055. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.052
  • Walenchok, S. C., Goldinger, S. D., & Hout, M. C. (2020). The confirmation and prevalence biases in visual search reflect separate underlying processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 46(3), 274. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000714
  • Wass, S. V., Forssman, L., & Leppänen, J. (2014). Robustness and precision: How data quality may influence key dependent variables in infant eye-tracker analyses. Infancy, 19(5), 427–460. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12055
  • Wedell, D. H., & Senter, S. M. (1997). Looking and weighting in judgment and choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(1), 41–64. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2692
  • Xiao, Y. J., Coppin, G., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Perceiving the world through group-colored glasses: A perceptual model of intergroup relations. Psychological Inquiry, 27(4), 255–274. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2016.1199221
  • Zhang, L., Wade, J., Swanson, A., Weitlauf, A., Warren, Z., & Sarkar, N. (2015, September). Cognitive state measurement from eye gaze analysis in an intelligent virtual reality driving system for autism intervention. In 2015 International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII) (pp. 532–538). Xi'an, China: IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/ACII.2015.7344621
  • Zhang, Y., Hedo, R., Rivera, A., Rull, R., Richardson, S., & Tu, X. M. (2019). Post hoc power analysis: Is it an informative and meaningful analysis? General Psychiatry, 32(4), e100069. https://doi.org/10.1136/gpsych-2019-100069

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.