119
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Intonation discrimination for tonal chord sequences in a priming paradigm:Effects of target predictability and musical expertise

, &
Pages 33-48 | Received 01 Feb 2021, Accepted 13 Aug 2021, Published online: 08 Sep 2021

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.
  • Bagwell, C., Sykes, R., & Giard, P. (2013). SoX sound eXchange (Version 14.4.2) [Computer software]. http://sox.sourceforge.net
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.
  • Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Reaction time and musical expectancy: Priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4), 403–410.
  • Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41(6), 519–524.
  • Bigand, E., Madurell, F., Tillmann, B., & Pineau, M. (1999). Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 184–197.
  • Bigand, E., & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(7), 1098–1107.
  • Bigand, E., Poulin, B., Tillmann, B., Madurell, F., & D’Adamo, D. A. (2003). Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 159–171.
  • Bigand, E., Tillmann, B., Poulin, B., D’Adamo, D. A., & Madurell, F. (2001). The effect of harmonic context on phoneme monitoring in vocal music. Cognition, 81(1), B11–B20.
  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.
  • Faroqi-Shah, Y., Slevc, L. R., Saxena, S., Fisher, S. J., & Pifer, M. (2019). Relationship between musical and language abilities in post-stroke aphasia. Aphasiology. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2019.1650159
  • Harrison, P., Collins, T., & Müllensiefen, D. (2017). Applying modern psychometric techniques to melodic discrimination testing: Item response theory, computerised adaptive testing, and automatic item generation. Scientific Reports, 7(3618). doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03586-z
  • ISO 389-8. (2004). Acoustics: Reference zero for the calibration of audiometric equipment—Part 8: Reference equivalent threshold sound pressure levels for pure tones and circumaural earphones. Geneva, Switzerland: International Organization for Standardization.
  • Justus, T., & Bharucha, J. J. (2001). Modularity in musical processing: The automaticity of harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 1000–1011.
  • Kingdom, F. A. A., & Prins, N. (2016). Psychophysics: A practical introduction. London, UK: Academic Press.
  • Larrouy-Maestri, P., Harrison, P. M. C., & Müllensiefen, D. (2019). The mistuning perception test: A new measurement instrument. Behavior Research Methods, 51(2), 663–675.
  • Lebrun-Guillaud, G., Tillmann, B., & Justus, T. (2008). Perception of tonal and temporal structures in chord sequences by patients with cerebellar damage. Music Perception, 25(4), 271–283.
  • Margulis, E. H., & Levine, W. H. (2006). Timbre priming effects and expectation in melody. Journal of New Music Research, 35(2), 175–182.
  • Marmel, F., & Tillmann, B. (2009). Tonal priming beyond tonics. Music Perception, 26(3), 211–221.
  • Martin, F. N., & Champlin, C. A. (2000). Reconsidering the limits of normal hearing. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 11, 64–66.
  • Micheyl, C., Delhommeau, K., Perrot, X., & Oxenham, A. J. (2006). Influence of musical and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination. Hearing Research, 219(1–2), 36–47.
  • Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Musil, J., & Stewart, L. (2014). The musicality of non-musicians: An index for assessing musical sophistication in the general population. PLoS ONE, 9(6). doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101091
  • Pearce, M. T. (2005). The construction and evaluation of statistical models of melodic structure in music perception and composition. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). City University, London.
  • Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2006). Expectation in melody: The influence of context and learning. Music Perception, 23(5), 377–405.
  • Poulin-Charronnat, B., Bigand, E., & Madurell, F. (2005). The influence of voice leading on harmonic priming. Music Perception, 22(4), 613–627.
  • Prins, N., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2018). Applying the model-comparison approach to test specific research hypotheses in psychophysical research using the Palamedes Toolbox. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1250). doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01250
  • Sears, D. R. W., Pearce, M. T., Spitzer, J., E, C. W., & McAdams, S. (2019). Expectations for tonal cadences: Sensory and cognitive priming effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(6), 1422–1438.
  • Slevc, L. R., Faroqi-Shah, Y., Saxena, S., & Okada, B. M. (2016). Preserved processing of musical structure in a person with agrammatic aphasia. Neurocase, 22(6), 505–511.
  • Tekman, H. G., & Bharucha, J. J. (1998). Implicit knowledge versus psychoacoustic similarity in priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(1), 252–260.
  • Tillmann, B., & Bigand, E. (2001). Global context effect in normal and scrambled musical sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(5), 1185–1196.
  • Tillmann, B., Bigand, E., Escoffier, N., & Lalitte, P. (2006). The influence of musical relatedness on timbre discrimination. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 343–358.
  • Tillmann, B., Janata, P., Birk, J., & Bharucha, J. J. (2003). The costs and benefits of tonal centers for chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 470–482.
  • Tillmann, B., Justus, T., & Bigand, E. (2008). Cerebellar patients demonstrate preserved implicit knowledge of association strengths in musical sequences. Brain and Cognition, 66(2), 161–167.
  • Tillmann, B., & Lebrun-Guillaud, G. (2006). Influence of tonal and temporal expectations on chord processing and on completion judgments of chord sequences. Psychological Research, 70(5), 345–358.
  • Tillmann, B., & Marmel, F. (2013). Musical expectations within chord sequences: Facilitation due to tonal stability without closure effects. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 23(1), 1–5.
  • Tillmann, B., Peretz, I., Bigand, E., & Gosselin, N. (2007). Harmonic priming in an amusic patient: The power of implicit tasks. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(6), 603–622.
  • Verbeten, J., & Sears, D. R. W. (2019). Pattern discovery using melodic-harmonic reductions of Bach chorales. Paper presented at the Biennial meeting of the Society for Music Perception and Cognition (SMPC), New York, NY.
  • West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models. A practical guide using statistical software. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.