270
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Development of Cognitive Shifting from Others’ Behavior in Young Children: A Near-infrared Spectroscopy Study

&
Pages 39-47 | Received 10 Oct 2019, Accepted 26 Dec 2019, Published online: 31 Dec 2019

References

  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326–339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x
  • Blair, C., Sulik, M., Willoughby, M., Mills‐Koonce, R., Petrill, S., Bartlett, C., & Greenberg, M. (2015). Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158met polymorphism interacts with early experience to predict executive functions in early childhood. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(7), 833–841. doi:10.1002/dev.21332
  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333
  • Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: The role of representation activation and maintenance [Empirical Study Quantitative Study]. Developmental Science, 11(3), 339–353. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00679.x.
  • Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(9), 571–578. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2015.07.003
  • Garon, N. M., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
  • Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Children’s developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83(1), 137–145. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01695.x
  • Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298. doi:10.1364/AO.48.00D280
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6(5), 449–467. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00300
  • Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2005). Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. Developmental Science, 8(1), 44–56. doi:10.1111/desc.2005.8.issue-1
  • Matsuda, G., & Hiraki, K. (2006). Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: A NIRS study of children. NeuroImage, 29(3), 706–711. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.019
  • Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2014). The development of co-representation effects in a joint task: Do children represent a co-actor? Cognition, 132(3), 269–279. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.04.008
  • Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2017). Individual differences in children’s corepresentation of self and other in joint action. Child Development, 88(3), 964–978. doi:10.1111/cdev.2017.88.issue-3
  • Moriguchi, Y. (2012). The effect of social observation on children’s inhibitory control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(2), 248–258. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.002
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2009). Neural origin of cognitive shifting in young children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(14), 6017–6021. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809747106
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2011). Longitudinal development of prefrontal function during early childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 153–162. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.12.004
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2014). Behavioral and neural differences during two versions of cognitive shifting tasks in young children and adults. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(4), 761–769. doi:10.1002/dev.21145
  • Moriguchi, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Itakura, S. (2010). Children perseverate to a human’s actions but not to a robot’s actions. Developmental Science, 13(1), 62–68. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00860.x
  • Moriguchi, Y., Lee, K., & Itakura, S. (2007). Social transmission of disinhibition in young children. Developmental Science, 10(4), 481–491. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00601.x
  • Moriguchi, Y., Minato, T., Ishiguro, H., Shinohara, I., & Itakura, S. (2010). Cues that trigger social transmission of disinhibition in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(2), 181–187. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.018
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Shinohara, I. (2018). Effect of the COMT Val158Met genotype on lateral prefrontal activations in young children. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12649. doi:10.1111/desc.12649
  • Munakata, Y., & Yerys, B. E. (2001). All together now: When dissociations between knowledge and action disappear. Psychological Science, 12(4), 335–337. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00361
  • Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8(1), 74–87. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
  • Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 353–367. doi:10.1111/tops.2009.1.issue-2
  • Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change card sort - Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive Development, 15(3), 347–365. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00021-6
  • Warneken, F., Gräfenhain, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children’s understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. Developmental Science, 15(1), 54–61. doi:10.1111/desc.2012.15.issue-1
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297–301. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.46
  • Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. (2013). NIH toolbox cognition battery (NIHTB-CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 16–33. doi:10.1111/mono.12032
  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 37–63. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90027-1
  • Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 1-+. doi:10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.