100
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

El reconocimiento temprano de la lengua materna: un estudio basado en la voz masculina

Early native-language recognition capacities: A study based on male-voices

, &
Pages 197-213 | Received 01 Oct 1999, Accepted 01 Feb 2000, Published online: 23 Jan 2014

Referencias

  • Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
  • Aslin, R. N., Pisoni, D. B. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1983). Auditory development and speech perception in infancy. En M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos (Eds.), Infancy and the biology of development (pp. 573–687). New York: Wiley.
  • Bahrick, L. E. & Pickens, J. N. (1988). Classification of bimodal English and Spanish language passages by infants. Infant behavior and Development, 11, 277–296.
  • Bosch, L. (1998). Bilingual exposure and some consequences on native-language recognition processes at four months. Proceedings 16th International Congress on Acoustics and 135th Meeting Acoustical Society of America (III, pp. 1599–1600). Seattle, Washington.
  • Bosch, L. & Sebastián-gallés, N. (1997a). Native-language recognition abilities in four-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65, 33–69.
  • Bosch, L. & Sebastián-gallés, N. (1997b). The role of prosody in infants's native-language discrimination abilities: the case of two phonologically close languages. Proceedings 5th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (pp. 231–234). Rhodes (Greece).
  • Bosch, L. & Sebastián-gallés, N. (1999). Early language discrimination capacities in infants from bilingual environments. Comunicación presentada en el VIII International Congress for the Study of Child Language. San Sebastián (Spain).
  • Bosch, L. & Sebastián-gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2, 29–49.
  • Christophe, A. & Morton, J. (1998). Is Dutch native English? Linguistic analysis by two-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 215–219.
  • Clifton, R. K. (1992). The development of spatial hearing in human infants. En L. A. Werner & E. W. Rubel (Eds.), Developmental Psychoacoustics (pp. 135–157). Washington D.C.: A.P.A.
  • Cooper, R. P. & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant directed-speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 1584–1595.
  • Cooper, R. P. & Aslin, R. N. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65, 1663–1677.
  • Cutler, A. & Mehler, J. (1993). The periodicity bias. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 103–108.
  • Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 51–62.
  • De Schonen, S., Deruelle, C., Mancini, J. & Pascalis, O. (1993). Hemispheric differences in face processing and brain maturation. En B. de Boysson-bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. Mcneilage & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental Neurocognition:Speech and Face processing during the first year of life (pp. 149–163). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  • Decasper, A. J. & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
  • Decasper, A. J. & Prescott, P. A. (1984). Human newborns' perception of male voices: preference, discrimination and reinforcing value. Developmental Psychobiology, 17 (5), 481–491.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G. & Houston, D. (1998). Faster orientation latencies toward native language in two-month old infants. Language and Speech, 41 (1), 21–43.
  • Fernald, A. (1984). The perceptual and affective salience of mothers' speech to infants. En L. Feagans, C. Garvey & R. Golinkoff (Eds.), The origins and growth of communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infants Behavior and Development, 8, 181–195.
  • Fernald, A. & Kuhl, P. K. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 279–293.
  • Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-bardies, B. & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477–501.
  • Fifer, W. P. & Moon, C. (1988). Audirory experience in the fetus. En W. Smotherman & S. Roberston (Eds.), Behavior of the fetus. West Caldwell, NJ: Telford Press.
  • Goldinger, S. D. (1996). Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1166–1183.
  • Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105, 251–279.
  • Jacobson, J. L., Boersma, D. C., Fields, R. B. & Olson, K. L. (1983). Paralinguistic features of adult speech to infants and small children. Child Development, 52 (2), 436–442.
  • Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I. & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: contingency learning, anticipatory looking and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3 (4), 335–344
  • Jusczyk, P. W. (1993). From general to language-specific capacities: the WRAPSA Model of how speech perception develops. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 3–28.
  • Jusczyk, P. W., Pisoni, D. B. & Mullenix, J. (1992). Some consequences of stimulus variability on speech processing by 2-month old infants. Cognition, 43, 253–291.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (1977). Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for vowel categories /a/ and /o/. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 61, Suplement 1, s 39 (A).
  • Kuhl, P. K. (1979). Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 1668–1679.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (1983). Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 263–285.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants exhibit a prototype effect for phoneme categories: monkeys do not. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 93–107.
  • Ladefoged, P. (1975). A course in phonetics. New York: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich.
  • Mehler, J. & Christophe, A. (1994). Language in the infant's mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, 346, 13–20.
  • Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Nazzi, T. & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (1996). Coping with linguistic diversity: the infant's viewpoint. En J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax (pp. 101–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
  • Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P. W., Lambertz, G., Halsted, G., Bertoncini, J. & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–178.
  • Moon, C., Cooper, R. & Fifer, W. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495–500.
  • Mullenix, J. W., Pisoni, D. B. & Martin, C. S. (1989). Some effects of talker variability on spoken word recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85, 365–378.
  • Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756–766.
  • Nespor, M. & Vogel, I. (1986). Prosodic Phonology. Dordrecht: Foris Publ.
  • Oliva, S. & Serra, P. (1990). Stress resolution at the phonological phrase level: some examples from Catalan and Spanish. Girona: Estudi General de Girona.
  • Otake, T. & Cutler, A. (1996). Phonological structure and language processing: Cross-linguistic studies. Berlin: Mouton/De Gruyter.
  • Serra, P. (1993). De la representació de l'accent. Llengua i literatura, 5, 417–443.
  • Serra, P. (1995). L'estructura prosòdica i l'accent. Caplletra, 19, 113–144.
  • Trainor, L. J. & Zacharias, C. A. (1998). Infants prefer higher-pitched singing. Infant Behavior and Development, 21 (4), 799–805.
  • Werker, J. F. & Mcleod, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230–246.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.