Publication Cover
Studies in Psychology
Estudios de Psicología
Volume 15, 1994 - Issue 51
1
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Aprendizaje implícito: tres aproximaciones a la cuestión del aprendizaje sin conciencia

Implicit learning: Three approaches to the matter of consciousless learning

, &
Pages 99-126 | Published online: 23 Jan 2014

Referencias

  • Berry, D. C. (en prensa). Implicit Learning: Twenty five years on. En C. Umiltà y M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing.
  • Berry, D. C., y Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36a, 209–231.
  • Berry, D. C., y Broadbent, D. E. (1987). The combination of explicit and implicit learning processes in task control. Psychological Research, 49, 7–15.
  • Berry, D. C., y Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Interactive tasks and the implicit-explicit distinction. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 251–272.
  • Berry, D. C., y Dienes, Z. (1991). The relationship between implicit memory and implicit learning. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 359–373.
  • Boakes, R. A. (1989). How one might find evidence for conditioning in adult humans? En T. Archer y L.G. Nilsson (Eds.). Aversion, Avoidance and Anxiety: Perspectives on learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987 Psychological Bulktin, 106, 265–289.
  • Bornstein, R. F. (1992). Subliminal mere exposure effects. En R. F. Bornstein y T. S. Pittman (Eds.). Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical and social perspectives. Nueva York: Guilford Press.
  • Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. En W. B. Weimer y D. S. Palermo (Eds.). Cognition and the symbolic processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 181–201.
  • Broadbent, D. E., y Aston, B. (1978). Human control of a simulated economic system. Ergonomics, 21, 1.035–1.043.
  • Broadbent, D. E.; Fitzgerald, P., y Broadbent, M. H. P. (1986). Implicit and explicit knowledge in the control of complex systems. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 33–50.
  • Brody, N. (1989). Unconscious learning of rules: Comment on Reber's analysis of implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 236–238.
  • Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. En E.R. Rosch y B. B. Lloyd (Eds.). Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Brooks, L. R., y Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 541–555.
  • Bruner, J. S.; Goodnow, J., y Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. Nueva York: Wiley.
  • Cleeremans, A. (1993a). Mechanisms of implicit learning: connectionist models of sequence processing. Cambridge. MIT Press.
  • Cleeremans, A. (1993b). Attention and awareness in sequence learning. Proceedings of the 15th. Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Cleeremans, A., y Mcclelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–253.
  • Cleeremans, A.; Servan-Schreiber, D., y Mcclelland, J. L. (1989). Finite state automata and simple recurrent networks. Neural Computation, 1, 372–381.
  • Cohen, A., Y Curtan, T. (en prensa). On tasks, knowledge, correlations and dissociations: A comment on Perruchet and Amorim. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition.
  • Cohen, A.; Ivry, R. I., y Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 17–30.
  • Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conception of memory storage, selective attention and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.
  • Cowan, N. (1993). Activation, Attention and short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 21, 162–167.
  • Curran, T., y Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 189–202.
  • Danks, J. H., y Gans, D. L. (1975). Acquisition and utilization of a rule structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 201–208.
  • Dawson, M. E., y Schell, A. M. (1985). Information Processing and human autonomic classical conditioning. En O. K. Ackles, J. R. Jennings y M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Advances in Psychophysiology, Vol. 1, Greenwich, Conn: JAI Press.
  • Dienes, Z. (1992). Connectionist and Memory-array models of artificial grammar learning. Cognitive Science, 16, 41–79.
  • Dienes, Z.; Broadbent, D., y Berry, D. (1991). Implicit and explicit knowledge bases in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 875–887.
  • Druhan, B., y Mathews, R. (1989). THIYOS: A classifier system model of implicit knowledge of artificial grammars. Proceedings of the eleventh annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Dulany, D. E.; Carlson, R. C., y Dewey, G. I. (1984). A case of syntatical learning and judgment: How conscious and how abstract? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 541–555.
  • Dulany, D. E.; Carlson, R. C., y Dewey, G. I. (1985). On consciousness in syntactical learning and judgment: A reply to Reber, Allen and Regan. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 25–32.
  • Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179–211.
  • Estes, W.K. (1986). Array models for category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 500–549.
  • Frensch, P. A.; Buchner, A., y Lin, J. (en prensa). Implicit learning and ambiguous serial transitions in the presence and absence of a distractor task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition.
  • Frensch, P. A., y Miner, C. S. (en prensa). Individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory and Cognition.
  • Gordon, P. C., y Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Implicit learning and generalization of the «mere exposure» effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 492–500.
  • Green, R. E. A., y Shanks, D. R. (en prensa). On the existence of independent implicit and explicit learning systems: An examination of some evidence. Memory and Cognition.
  • Greenwald, A. G. (1992). Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist, 47, 766–779.
  • Hartman, M.; Knopman, D. S., y Nissen, M. J. (1989). Implicit learning of new verbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1.070–1.082.
  • Hasher, L., y Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortfull processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.
  • Hasher, L., y Zacks, R. T. (1984). Automatic processes of fundamental information. American Psychologist, 39, 1.372–1.388.
  • Hayes, N. A., y Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Two modes of learning for interactive tasks. Cognition, 28, 249–276.
  • Howard, D. V., y Howard, J. H. (1989). Age differences in learning serial patterns: Direct versus indirect measures. Psychology and Aging, 4, 357–364.
  • Howard, J. H.; Mutter, S. A., y Howard, D. V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1.029–1.039.
  • Hunt, E., y Lansman, M. (1986). Unified model of attention and problem solving. Psychological Review, 93, 446–461.
  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.
  • Jennings, P. J., y Keele, S. W. (1990). A computational model of attentional requirements in sequence learning. Proceedings of the 12th. Annual Conference of The Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Jiménez, L.; Lorda, M. J., y Méndez, C. (1993). Condicionamiento con estímulos enmascarados: efectos de priming como evidencia de aprendizaje no consciente. Estudiosd Psicología, 49, 33–50.
  • Jiménez, L.; Méndez, C., y Cleeremans, A. (en preparación). Implicit learning and awareness of the structure of event sequences.
  • Jiménez, L.; Méndez, C., y Lorda, M. J. (1993). Aprendizaje implícito, atención y conciencia en tareas de tiempo de reacción serial. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 46, 245–255.
  • Jiménez, L., y Méndez, C. (1993). Funcionalidad de la conciencia y procesamiento no consciente. Manuscrito enviado para publicación.
  • Jordan, M. I. (1986). Attractor dynamics and parallelism in a conectionist sequential machine. Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Keele, S. W., y Jennings, P. J. (1992). Attention in the representation of sequence: Experiment and Theory. Human Movement studies, 11, 125–138.
  • Klahr, D.; Dunbar, K., y Fay, A. L. (1990). Designing good experiments to test bad hypotheses. En J. Shrager y P. Langley (Eds.), Computational models of scientific discovery and theory formation. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
  • Klayman, J., y Ha, Y. (1989). Hypothesis testing in rule discovery: Strategy, structure and content. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 596–604.
  • Knopman, D. (1991). Unaware learning versus preserved learning in parmacologic amnesia: similarities and difference s. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 1.017–1.029.
  • Knopman, D. S., y Nissen, M. J. (1987). Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 31, 784–788.
  • Knopman, D. S., y Nissen, M. J. (1991). Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Neuropsychologia, 29, 245–254.
  • Lewicki, P.; Czyzewska, M., y Hoffman, H. (1987). Unconscious acquisition of complex procedual knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory arni Cognition, 13, 523–530.
  • Lewicki, P.; Hill, T., y Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24–37.
  • Marescaux, P. J., y Karnas, G. (1991). The implicit versus explicit knowledge distinction revisited': when finding associations between verbalizable knowledge and some performance criteria. Informe 4PR3GK del proyecto KAUDYTE (ESPRIT BRA 3219). Faculté des Sciences Psychologiques et Pédagogiques. Université Libre de Bruxelles.
  • Marescaux, P. J.; Luc, F., y Karnas, G. (1989). Modes d'apprentissage selectif et nonselectif et connaissances acquises au control d'un processes. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 9, 239–264.
  • Mathews, R. C. (1990). Abstractness of implicit grammar knowledge: Comments on Perruchet and Pacteau's analysis of synthetic grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 412–416.
  • Mathews, R. C. (1991). The forgetting algorithm: How fragmentary knowledge of exemplars can abstract knowledge. Journal of Experimental Pychology: General, 120, 117–119.
  • Mathews, R. C.; Buss, R. R.; Stanley, W. B.; Blanchard-Fields, F.; Cho, J. R., y Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit processes in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1.083–1.100.
  • Mathews, R. C.; Buss, R. R.; Stanley, W. B., y Chinn, R. (1988). Analysis of individual learning curves in a concept discovery task: Relations among task performance, verbalizable knowledge, and hypothesis revision strategies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 135–165.
  • Mcandrews, M. P., y Moscovitch, M. (1985). Ruled-based and exemplar-based classification in artificial grammar learning. Memory & Cognition, 13, 469–475.
  • Merikle, P. M., y Reingold, E. M. (1992). Measuring unconscious perceptual processes. En R. F. Bornstein y T.S. Pittman (Eds.). Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical and social perspectives. Nueva York: Guilford Press.
  • Millward, R. B. (1981). Models of concept formation. En R.S. Snow, P.A. Federico y W.E. Montague (Eds.). Aptitude, learning and instruction: Cognitive process analysis. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Nissen, M. J., y Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.
  • Nissen, M. J.; Knopman, D., y Schacter, D. L. (1987). Neurochemical dissociation of memory systems. Neurology, 3, 7, 789–794.
  • Nissen, M. J.; Wit, T. Ingham, D., y Hartman, M. (1989). Explicit and implicit remembering: When is learning preserved in amnesia? Neuropsychologia, 27, 341–352.
  • Öhman, A.; Dimberg, U., y Esteves, F. (1989). Preattentive activation of aversive emotions. En T. Archer y L. G. Nilsson (Eds.). Aversion, Avoidance and Anxiety: Perspectives on learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Perruchet, P. (en prensa). Learning from complex rule-governed environments: On the proper functions of nonconscious and conscious processes. En C. Umiltà y M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing. Cambridge, MA. Bradford.
  • Perruchet, P., y Amorim, P. A. (1992). Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: evidence against dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 785–800.
  • Perruchet, P.; Gallego, J., y Pacteau, CH. (1992). A reinterpretation of some earlier evidence for abstractiveness of implicitly acquired knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44 a, 193–210.
  • Perruchet, P.; Gallego, L., y Savy, I. (1990). A critical reappraisal of the evidence for unconscious abstraction o deterministic rules in complex experimental situations. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 493–516.
  • Perruchet, P., y Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: Implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 264–275.
  • Perruchet, P., y Pacteau, C. (1991). The implicit acquisition of abstract knowledge about atificial grammar: some methodological and conceptual issues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 112–116.
  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.
  • Reber, A. S. (1969). Tansfer of syntactic structures in synthetic languages. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 115–119.
  • Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of artificial languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 88–94.
  • Reber, A. S. (1989a). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.
  • Reber, A. S. (1989b). More thoughts on the unconscious: reply to brody and to Lewicki and Hill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 242–244.
  • Reber, A. S. (1990). On the primacy of the implicit: Comment on Perruchet and Pacteau. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 340–342.
  • Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and Tacit knowledge. Oxford University Press.
  • Reber, A. S., y Allen, R. (1978). Analogy and abstraction strategies in synthetic glammar learning: A functionalist interpretation. Cognition, 6, 189–221.
  • Reber, A. S.; Allen, R., y Regan, S. (1985). Syntactical learning and judgment, still unconscious and still abstract: Comment on Dulany, Carlson and Dewey. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 17–24.
  • Reber, A. S.; Kassin, S. M.; Lewis, S., y Cantor, G. (1980). On the relations between implicit and explicit models in the learning of a complex rule structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human learning and Memory, 6, 492–502.
  • Reber, A. S., y Lewis, S. (1977). Toward a theory of implicit learning: The analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333–361.
  • Reingold, E. M., y Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 563–575.
  • Roussel, L. G.; Mathews, R. C., y Druhan, B. B. (1990). Rule induction and interference in the absence of feed-back: a classifier system model. Proceedings of the 12th. Annual Conference of The Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Rumelhart, D. E.; Mcclelland, J. L., y el grupo de investigación PDP (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: (Vol. 1). Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.
  • Rumelart, D. E.; Hinton, G. E., y Williams, R. J. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. En D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland y el grupo de investigación PDP (Eds.). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 1). Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.
  • Sanderson, P. M. (1989). Verbalizable knowledge and skilled task performance: association, dissociation, and mental models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 729–747.
  • Sanders, R. E.; González, E. G.; Murphy, M. D.; Liddle, C. L., y Vitina, J. R. (1987). Frequency of occurrence and the criteria for automatic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 241–250.
  • Seger, C.A. (1993). Implicit learning. Manuscrito enviado para publicación.
  • Servan-Schreiber, E., y Anderson, J. R (1990). Learning artificial grammars with competitive chunking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 592–608.
  • Shanks, D. R., y St. John, M. F. (1992). Characteristics of disociable learning systems. Technical Report No 9203. Department of Cognitive Science, University of California San Diego. La Jolla, CA.
  • Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1.061–1.069.
  • Stadler, M. A. (1992). Statistical structure and implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 318–327.
  • Stanley, W. B.; Mathews, R. C.; Buss, R. R., y Kotlercope, S. (1989). Insight without awareness: On the interaction of verbalizatio n, instruction and practice in a simulated process control task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41a, 553–577.
  • Vokey, J. R., y Brooks, L. R. (1992). Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammars. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 328–344.
  • Willingham, D. B.; Nissen, M. J., y Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1.047–1.060.
  • Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.