185
Views
35
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Androgen responsiveness to competition in humans: the role of cognitive variables

&
Pages 19-32 | Published online: 11 Feb 2014

References

  • Adkins-Regan E. Hormonal organization and activation: evolutionary implications and questions. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2012;176(3):279–285.
  • Laue L, Cutler GB Jr. The adrenal cortex. In: Bittar EE, Bittar N, editors. Principles of Medical Biology: Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1997:227–254.
  • Longcope C. Adrenal and gonadal androgen secretion in normal females. Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1986;15(2):213–228.
  • Burger HG. Androgen production in women. Fertil Steril. 2002; 77 Suppl 4:S3–S5.
  • Schmidt KL, Pradhan DS, Shah AH, Charlier TD, Chin EH, Soma KK. Neurosteroids, immunosteroids, and the Balkanization of endocrinology. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2008;157(3):266–274.
  • Reddy DS. Neurosteroids: endogenous role in the human brain and therapeutic potentials. Prog Brain Res. 2010;186:113–137.
  • Girdler SS, Klatzkin R. Neurosteroids in the context of stress: implications for depressive disorders. Pharmacol Ther. 2007;116(1):125–139.
  • Oliveira RF. Social modulation of androgens in vertebrates: mechanisms and function. Adv Stud Behav. 2004;34:165–239.
  • Mazur A. A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces. 1985;64(2):377–402.
  • Mazur A, Booth A. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav Brain Sci. 1998;21(3):353–363; discussion 363–397.
  • Eisenegger C, Haushofer J, Fehr E. The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(6):263–271.
  • Josephs RA, Mehta PH, Carré JM. Gender and social environment modulate the effects of testosterone on social behavior: comment on Eisenegger et al. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(11):509–510; author reply 510.
  • Wingfield JC, Hegner RE, Dufty AM Jr, Ball GF. The “Challenge Hypothesis”: theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies. Am Nat. 1990;136(6):829–846.
  • Archer J. Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(3):319–345.
  • Hirschenhauser K, Oliveira RF. Social modulation of androgens in male vertebrates: meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Anim Behav. 2006;71(2):265–277.
  • Scott MP. Resource defense and juvenile hormone: the “challenge hypothesis” extended to insects. Horm Behav. 2006;49(3):276–281.
  • Goymann W, Landys MM, Wingfield JC. Distinguishing seasonal androgen responses from male-male androgen responsiveness-revisiting the Challenge Hypothesis. Horm Behav. 2007;51(4):463–476.
  • Goymann W. Social modulation of androgens in male birds. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2009;163(1–2):149–157.
  • Wingfield JC, Lynn S, Soma KK. Avoiding the ‘costs’ of testosterone: ecological bases of hormone-behavior interactions. Brain Behav Evol. 2001;57(5):239–251.
  • Hau M. Regulation of male traits by testosterone: implications for the evolution of vertebrate life histories. Bioessays. 2007;29(2):133–144.
  • Schroderus E, Jokinen I, Koivula M, et al. Intra- and intersexual trade-offs between testosterone and immune system: Implications for sexual and sexually antagonistic selection. Am Nat. 2010;176(4):E90–E97.
  • Hsu Y, Earley RL, Wolf LL. Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2006;81(1):33–74.
  • Oyegbile TO, Marler CA. Winning fights elevates testosterone levels in California mice and enhances future ability to win fights. Horm Behav. 2005;48(3):259–267.
  • Fuxjager MJ, Marler CA. How and why the winner effect forms: influences of contest environment and species differences. Behav Ecol. 2009;21(1):37–45.
  • Gleason ED, Fuxjager MJ, Oyegbile TO, Marler CA. Testosterone release and social context: when it occurs and why. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30(4):460–469.
  • Oliveira RF. Social behavior in context: Hormonal modulation of behavioral plasticity and social competence. Integr Comp Biol. 2009;49(4):423–440.
  • Trainor BC, Bird IM, Marler CA. Opposing hormonal mechanisms of aggression revealed through short-lived testosterone manipulations and multiple winning experiences. Horm Behav. 2004;45(2):115–121.
  • Mazur A, Lamb TA. Testosterone, status, and mood in human males. Horm Behav. 1980;14(3):236–246.
  • Elias M. Serum cortisol, testosterone, and testosterone-binding globulin responses to competitive fighting in human males. Aggress Behav. 1981;7(3):215–224.
  • Booth A, Shelley G, Mazur A, Tharp G, Kittok R. Testosterone, and winning and losing in human competition. Horm Behav. 1989;23(4):556–571.
  • Gladue BA, Boechler M, McCaul KD. Hormonal response to competition in human males. Aggress Behav. 1989;15(6):409–422.
  • Mazur A, Booth A, Dabbs JM Jr. Testosterone and chess competition. Soc Psychol Q. 1992;55(1):70–77.
  • McCaul KD, Gladue BA, Joppa M. Winning, losing, mood, and testosterone. Horm Behav. 1992;26(4):486–504.
  • Mazur A, Susman EJ, Edelbrock S. Sex difference in testosterone response to a video game contest. Evol Hum Behav. 1997;18(5):317–326.
  • Bernhardt PC, Dabbs JM, Fielden JA, Lutter CD. Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and losing among fans at sporting events. Physiol Behav. 1998;65(1):59–62.
  • Gonzalez-Bono E, Salvador A, Serrano MA, Ricarte J. Testosterone, cortisol, and mood in a sports team competition. Horm Behav. 1999;35(1):55–62.
  • Schultheiss OC, Campbell KL, McClelland DC. Implicit power motivation moderates men’s testosterone responses to imagined and real dominance success. Horm Behav. 1999;36(3):234–241.
  • Suay F, Salvador A, González-Bono E, et al. Effects of competition and its outcome on serum testosterone, cortisol and prolactin. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24(5):551–566.
  • González-Bono E, Salvador A, Ricarte J, Serrano MA, Arnedo M. Testosterone and attribution of successful competition. Aggress Behav. 2000;26(3):235–240.
  • Serrano MA, Salvador A, González-Bono E, Sanchís C, Suay F. Hormonal responses to competition. Psicothema. 2000;12(3):440–444.
  • Filaire E, Maso F, Sagnol M, Ferrand C, Lac G. Anxiety, hormonal responses, and coping during a judo competition. Aggress Behav. 2001;27(1):55–63.
  • Bateup HS, Booth A, Shirtcliff EA, Granger DA. Testosterone, cortisol, and women’s competition. Evol Hum Behav. 2002;23(3):181–192.
  • Schultheiss OC, Rohde W. Implicit power motivation predicts men’s testosterone changes and implicit learning in a contest situation. Horm Behav. 2002;41(2):195–202.
  • Wagner JD, Flinn MV, England BG. Hormonal response to competition among male coalitions. Evol Hum Behav. 2002;23(6):437–442.
  • Kivlighan KT, Granger DA, Booth A. Gender differences in testosterone and cortisol response to competition. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30(1):58–71.
  • Schultheiss OC, Wirth MM, Torges CM, Pang JS, Villacorta MA, Welsh KM. Effects of implicit power motivation on men’s and women’s implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(1):174–188.
  • Edwards DA, Wetzel K, Wyner DR. Intercollegiate soccer: saliva cortisol and testosterone are elevated during competition, and testosterone is related to status and social connectedness with team mates. Physiol Behav. 2006;87(1):135–143.
  • Josephs RA, Sellers JG, Newman ML, Mehta PH. The mismatch effect: when testosterone and status are at odds. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90(6):999–1013.
  • Mehta PH, Josephs RA. Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again. Horm Behav. 2006;50(5):684–692.
  • Parmigiani S, Bartolomucci A. In judo, Randori (free fight) and Kata (highly ritualized fight) differentially change plasma cortisol, testosterone, and interleukin levels in male participants. Aggress Behav. 2006;32(5):481–489.
  • Stanton SJ, Schultheiss OC. Basal and dynamic relationships between implicit power motivation and estradiol in women. Horm Behav. 2007;52(5):571–580.
  • van Anders SM, Watson NV. Effects of ability- and chance-determined competition outcome on testosterone. Physiol Behav. 2007;90(4):634–642.
  • Carré JM, McCormick CM. Aggressive behavior and change in salivary testosterone concentrations predict willingness to engage in a competitive task. Horm Behav. 2008;54(3):403–409.
  • Mehta PH, Jones AC, Josephs RA. The social endocrinology of dominance: basal testosterone predicts cortisol changes and behavior following victory and defeat. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;94(6):1078–1093.
  • Carré JM. No place like home: testosterone responses to victory depend on game location. Am J Hum Biol. 2009;21(3):392–394.
  • Carré JM, Putnam SK, McCormick CM. Testosterone responses to competition predict future aggressive behaviour at a cost to reward in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(4):561–570.
  • Edwards DA, O’Neal JL. Oral contraceptives decrease saliva testosterone but do not affect the rise in testosterone associated with athletic competition. Horm Behav. 2009;56(2):195–198.
  • Hamilton LD, van Anders SM, Cox DN, Watson NV. The effect of competition on salivary testosterone in elite female athletes. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2009;4(4):538–542.
  • Mehta PH, Wuehrmann EV, Josephs RA. When are low testosterone levels advantageous? The moderating role of individual versus intergroup competition. Horm Behav. 2009;56(1):158–162.
  • Oliveira T, Gouveia MJ, Oliveira RF. Testosterone responsiveness to winning and losing experiences in female soccer players. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(7):1056–1064.
  • Pound N, Penton-Voak IS, Surridge AK. Testosterone responses to competition in men are related to facial masculinity. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276(1654):153–159.
  • Stanton SJ, Beehner JC, Saini EK, Kuhn CM, Labar KS. Dominance, politics, and physiology: voters’ testosterone changes on the night of the 2008 United States presidential election. PLoS One. 2009;4(10):e7543.
  • Carré JM, Gilchrist JD, Morrissey MD, McCormick CM. Motivational and situational factors and the relationship between testosterone dynamics and human aggression during competition. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(2):346–353.
  • Edwards DA, Kurlander LS. Women’s intercollegiate volleyball and tennis: effects of warm-up, competition, and practice on saliva levels of cortisol and testosterone. Horm Behav. 2010;58(4):606–613.
  • Oxford J, Ponzi D, Geary DC. Hormonal responses differ when playing violent video games against an ingroup and outgroup. Evol Hum Behav. 2010;31(3):201–209.
  • Steiner ET, Barchard KA, Meana M, Hadi F, Gray PB. The deal on testosterone responses to poker competition. Curr Psychol. 2010;29(1):45–51.
  • van der Meij L, Buunk AP, Almela M, Salvador A. Testosterone responses to competition: The opponent’s psychological state makes it challenging. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(2):330–335.
  • Slatcher RB, Mehta PH, Josephs RA. Testosterone and self-reported dominance interact to influence human mating behavior. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2011;2(5):531–539.
  • Costa R, Salvador A. Associations between success and failure in a face-to-face competition and psychobiological parameters in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(11):1780–1790.
  • Jiménez M, Aguilar R, Alvero-Cruz JR. Effects of victory and defeat on testosterone and cortisol response to competition: evidence for same response patterns in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(9):1577–1581.
  • Trumble BC, Cummings D, von Rueden C, et al. Physical competition increases testosterone among Amazonian forager-horticulturalists: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279(1739):2907–2912.
  • van der Meij L, Almela M, Hidalgo V, et al. Testosterone and cortisol release among Spanish soccer fans watching the 2010 World Cup final. PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e34814.
  • Zilioli S, Watson NV. The hidden dimensions of the competition effect: basal cortisol and basal testosterone jointly predict changes in salivary testosterone after social victory in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(11):1855–1865.
  • Carré JM, Campbell JA, Lozoya E, Goetz SM, Welker KM. Changes in testosterone mediate the effect of winning on subsequent aggressive behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(10):2034–2041.
  • Denson TF, Mehta PH, Ho Tan D. Endogenous testosterone and cortisol jointly influence reactive aggression in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(3):416–424.
  • Oliveira GA, Uceda S, Oliveira T, Fernandes A, Garcia-Marques T, Oliveira RF. Threat perception and familiarity moderate the androgen response to competition in women. Front Psychol. 2013;4:389.
  • Dabbs JM, de La Rue D. Salivary testosterone measurements among women: relative magnitude of circadian and menstrual cycles. Horm Res. 1991;35(5):182–184.
  • Liening SH, Stanton SJ, Saini EK, Schultheiss OC. Salivary testosterone, cortisol, and progesterone: two-week stability, interhormone correlations, and effects of time of day, menstrual cycle, and oral contraceptive use on steroid hormone levels. Physiol Behav. 2010;99(1):8–16.
  • Salvador A. Coping with competitive situations in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(1):195–205.
  • Salvador A, Costa R. Coping with competition: neuroendocrine responses and cognitive variables. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33(2):160–170.
  • Kemper TD. Fantasy, females, sexuality, and testosterone. Behav Brain Sci. 1998;21(03):378–379.
  • van Anders SM, Watson NV. Social neuroendocrinology: Effects of social contexts and behaviors on sex steroids in humans. Hum Nat. 2006;17(2):212–237.
  • Grant VJ, France JT. Dominance and testosterone in women. Biol Psychol. 2001;58(1):41–47.
  • Cashdan E. Hormones and competitive aggression in women. Aggress Behav. 2003;29:107–115.
  • Wirth MM, Schultheiss OC. Basal testosterone moderates responses to anger faces in humans. Physiol Behav. 2007;90(2–3):496–505.
  • Edwards DA. Competition and testosterone. Horm Behav. 2006;50:681–683.
  • Klinesmith J, Kasser T, McAndrew FT. Guns, testosterone, and aggression: an experimental test of a mediational hypothesis. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(7):568–571.
  • Mendl M, Burman OH, Paul ES. An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277(1696):2895–2904.
  • Lazarus RS. Cognition and motivation in emotion. Am Psychol. 1991;46(4):352–367.
  • Scherer KR. Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In: Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2001:92–120.
  • Oliveira RF, Carneiro LA, Canário AV. Behavioural endocrinology: no hormonal response in tied fights. Nature. 2005;437(7056):207–208.
  • Hirschenhauser K, Taborsky M, Oliveira T, Canario AVM, Oliveira RF. A test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’ in cichlid fish: simulated partner and territory intruder experiments. Anim Behav. 2004;68(4):741–750.
  • Hirschenhauser K, Wittek M, Johnston P, Möstl E. Social context rather than behavioral output or winning modulates post-conflict testosterone responses in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Physiol Behav. 2008;95(3):457–463.
  • Neave N, Wolfson S. Testosterone, territoriality, and the ‘home advantage’. Physiol Behav. 2003;78(2):269–275.
  • Carré J, Muir C, Belanger J, Putnam SK. Pre-competition hormonal and psychological levels of elite hockey players: relationship to the “home advantage”. Physiol Behav. 2006;89(3):392–398.
  • Stanton SJ, Schultheiss OC. The hormonal correlates of implicit power motivation. J Res Pers. 2009;43(5):942.
  • Hartgens F, Kuipers H. Effects of androgenic-anabolic steroids in athletes. Sports Med. 2004;34(8):513–554.
  • Husak JF, Irschick DJ. Steroid use and human performance: Lessons for integrative biologists. Integr Comp Biol. 2009;49(4):354–364.
  • Wood RI, Stanton SJ. Testosterone and sport: current perspectives. Horm Behav. 2012;61(1):147–155.
  • Bhasin S, Woodhouse L, Storer TW. Proof of the effect of testosterone on skeletal muscle. J Endocrinol. 2001;170(1):27–38.
  • Adkins-Regan E. Hormones and Animal Social Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2005.
  • Bradley BP, Mogg K, Falla SJ, Hamilton LR. Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: manipulation of stimulus duration. Cogn Emot. 1998;12(6):737–753.
  • Bradley BP, Mogg K, Millar NH. Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety. Cogn Emotion. 2000;14(6):789–808.
  • Frye CA, Seliga AM. Testosterone increases analgesia, anxiolysis, and cognitive performance of male rats. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2001;1(4):371–381.
  • Aikey JL, Nyby JG, Anmuth DM, James PJ. Testosterone rapidly reduces anxiety in male house mice (Mus musculus). Horm Behav. 2002;42(4):448–460.
  • van Honk J, Peper JS, Schutter DJ. Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(3):218–225.
  • Hermans EJ, Putman P, Baas JM, Koppeschaar HP, van Honk J. A single administration of testosterone reduces fear-potentiated startle in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(9):872–874.
  • Hermans EJ, Putman P, Baas JM, Gecks NM, Kenemans JL, van Honk J. Exogenous testosterone attenuates the integrated central stress response in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(8–10):1052–1061.
  • van Honk J, Schutter DJ. Testosterone reduces conscious detection of signals serving social correction: implications for antisocial behavior. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(8):663–667.
  • van Honk J, Tuiten A, Hermans E, et al. A single administration of testosterone induces cardiac accelerative responses to angry faces in healthy young women. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115(1):238–242.
  • van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Hermans EJ, Putman P, Tuiten A, Koppeschaar H. Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(7):937–943.
  • van Honk J, Tuiten A, Verbaten R, et al. Correlations among salivary testosterone, mood, and selective attention to threat in humans. Horm Behav. 1999;36(1):17–24.
  • van Honk J, Tuiten A, van den Hout M, et al. Conscious and preconscious selective attention to social threat: different neuroendocrine response patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25(6):577–591.
  • Bos PA, Terburg D, van Honk J. Testosterone decreases trust in socially naive humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(22):9991–9995.
  • Hermans EJ, Putman P, van Honk J. Testosterone administration reduces empathetic behavior: a facial mimicry study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(7):859–866.
  • van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Bos PA, Kruijt AW, Lentjes EG, Baron-Cohen S. Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(8):3448–3452.
  • Berenbaum SA, Bryk KK, Nowak N, Quigley CA, Moffat S. Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology. 2009;150(11):5119–5124.
  • Hermans EJ, Ramsey NF, van Honk J. Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(3):263–270.
  • van Wingen GA, Zylicz SA, Pieters S, et al. Testosterone increases amygdala reactivity in middle-aged women to a young adulthood level. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(3):539–547.
  • Derntl B, Windischberger C, Robinson S, et al. Amygdala activity to fear and anger in healthy young males is associated with testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(5):687–693.
  • Manuck SB, Marsland AL, Flory JD, Gorka A, Ferrell RE, Hariri AR. Salivary testosterone and a trinucleotide (CAG) length polymorphism in the androgen receptor gene predict amygdala reactivity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):94–104.
  • Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(5):242–249.
  • Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, Phan KL. Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(2):168–178.
  • van Wingen G, Mattern C, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar J, Fernández G. Testosterone reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):105–113.
  • Mehta PH, Beer J. Neural mechanisms of the testosterone-aggression relation: the role of orbitofrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(10):2357–2368.
  • Hermans EJ, Bos PA, Ossewaarde L, Ramsey NF, Fernández G, van Honk J. Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response during reward anticipation in healthy women. Neuroimage. 2010;52(1):277–283.
  • Liben LS, Susman EJ, Finkelstein JW, et al. The effects of sex steroids on spatial performance: a review and an experimental clinical investigation. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(2):236–253.
  • Silverman I, Kastuk D, Choi J, Phillips K. Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24(8):813–822.
  • Thilers PP, Macdonald SW, Herlitz A. The association between endogenous free testosterone and cognitive performance: a population-based study in 35 to 90 year-old men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(5):565–576.
  • Van Strien JW, Weber RF, Burdorf A, Bangma C. Higher free testosterone level is associated with faster visual processing and more flanker interference in older men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(4):546–554.
  • Moffat SD, Hampson E. A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1996;21(3):323–337.
  • Yonker JE, Eriksson E, Nilsson LG, Herlitz A. Negative association of testosterone on spatial visualization in 35 to 80 year old men. Cortex. 2006;42(3):376–386.
  • Matousek RH, Sherwin BB. Sex steroid hormones and cognitive functioning in healthy, older men. Horm Behav. 2010;57(3):352–359.
  • Postma A, Meyer G, Tuiten A, van Honk J, Kessels RP, Thijssen J. Effects of testosterone administration on selective aspects of object-location memory in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25(6):563–575.
  • Aleman A, Bronk E, Kessels RP, Koppeschaar HP, van Honk J. A single administration of testosterone improves visuospatial ability in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(5):612–617.
  • Janowsky JS, Oviatt SK, Orwoll ES. Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108(2):325–332.
  • Cherrier MM, Asthana S, Plymate S, et al. Testosterone supplementation improves spatial and verbal memory in healthy older men. Neurology. 2001;57(1):80–88.
  • O’Connor DB, Archer J, Hair WM, Wu FC. Activational effects of testosterone on cognitive function in men. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(13):1385–1394.
  • Newman ML, Sellers JG, Josephs RA. Testosterone, cognition, and social status. Horm Behav. 2005;47(2):205–211.
  • Camerer CF. Progress in behavioral game theory. J Econ Perspect. 1997;11(4):167–188.
  • Burnham TC. High-testosterone men reject low ultimatum game offers. Proc Biol Sci. 2007;274(1623):2327–2330.
  • Zak PJ, Kurzban R, Ahmadi S, et al. Testosterone administration decreases generosity in the ultimatum game. PLoS One. 2009;4(12):e8330.
  • Eisenegger C, Naef M, Snozzi R, Heinrichs M, Fehr E. Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour. Nature. 2010;463(7279):356–359.
  • Eisenegger C, von Eckardstein A, Fehr E, von Eckardstein S. Pharmacokinetics of testosterone and estradiol gel preparations in healthy young men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(2):171–178.
  • van Honk J, Montoya ER, Bos PA, van Vugt M, Terburg D. New evidence on testosterone and cooperation. Nature. 2012;485(7399):E4–E5; discussion E5–E6.
  • Apicella CL, Dreber A, Campbell B, Gray PB, Hoffman M, Little AC. Testosterone and financial risk preferences. Evol Hum Behav. 2008;29(6):384–390.
  • Sapienza P, Zingales L, Maestripieri D. Gender differences in financial risk aversion and career choices are affected by testosterone. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(36):15268–15273.
  • Joel D, Tarrasch R. The risk of a wrong conclusion: on testosterone and gender differences in risk aversion and career choices. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(5):E19; author reply E20.
  • Stanton SJ, Mullette-Gillman OA, McLaurin RE, et al. Low- and high-testosterone individuals exhibit decreased aversion to economic risk. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(4):447–453.
  • Viau V. Functional cross-talk between the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal and -adrenal axes. J Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14(6):506–513.
  • Popma A, Vermeiren R, Geluk CA, et al. Cortisol moderates the relationship between testosterone and aggression in delinquent male adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(3):405–411.
  • Terburg D, Morgan B, van Honk J. The testosterone-cortisol ratio: A hormonal marker for proneness to social aggression. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2009;32(4):216–223.
  • Mehta PH, Josephs RA. Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Horm Behav. 2010;58(5):898–906.
  • Gettler LT, McDade TW, Kuzawa CW. Cortisol and testosterone in Filipino young adult men: evidence for co-regulation of both hormones by fatherhood and relationship status. Am J Hum Biol. 2011;23(5):609–620.
  • Kempenaers B, Peters A, Foerster K. Sources of individual variation in plasma testosterone levels. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363(1497):1711–1723.