13,127
Views
13
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
TEACHER EDUCATION & DEVELOPMENT

The impact of metacognitive strategy training and critical thinking awareness-raising on reading comprehension

, & | (Reviewing editor)
Article: 1720946 | Received 31 Jul 2019, Accepted 20 Jan 2020, Published online: 20 Feb 2020

Abstract

The present quasi-experimental research aimed to examine the differential effect of metacognitive strategy training and critical thinking awareness-raising on EFL learners’ reading comprehension of general reading (GR), cause and effect (CE), and argumentative (ARG) texts. The study set to investigate the extent to which raising Iranian EFL learners’ awareness of critical thinking skills and three sets of metacognitive reading strategies including global, problem-solving, and support may influence the three groups of participants’ reading comprehension. To this end, a purposeful sample of 54 intermediate male Iranian EFL learners attending three classes was recruited from a pool of 85 English learners and was randomly assigned as two experimental groups receiving metacognitive training (MCG) and critical thinking awareness-raising (CTG) and a control group (CG). The research data were gleaned using a researcher-made piloted 54-itme reading comprehension test. The results of the paired sample t-test analyses demonstrated that both MCG and CTG made significant improvements in comprehending GR and ARG texts from the pre-test to the post-test and that both outperformed the CG in comprehending GR and ARG post-tests. With regard to CE comprehension, the only significant difference was observed between the MCG and the CG. The findings offer a number of pedagogical implications.

PUBLIC INTEREST STATEMENT

Reading comprehension is undoubtedly considered as a cognitive and affective skill the development of which entails consistent on practical processing of a wide range of input on the one hand and the effectiveness of processing mechanisms that enable the learner to convert written language to thoughts and ideas on the other hand. This study focuses metacognitive strategies and critical thinking skills that help EFL learners to comprehend a text.

This study reminds EFL learners that when they face difficulty in reading comprehension, there is no need to stop reading and they can try to infer the meaning of the text by using metacognitive strategies and critical thinking skills. In addition, using metacognitive strategies raises the level of EFL learners’ awareness. The findings of the study have benefits for the EFL teachers in terms of developing learners’ reading comprehension through giving explicit instruction on metacognitive strategies and critical thinking skills.

1. Introduction

Reading comprehension has long been emphasized as an important skill in foreign language learning. The overriding role attributed to reading has been substantiated with respect to the potential practice activities it can provide for widening learners’ repertoire of words and structures or the opportunities it offers for exposure to incessant input. The conventional mechanical views of reading comprehension as a passive receptive skill have been superseded by the cognitive and interactive perspective which envisage it as an active information processing skill in which readers link the written input to their perceptual and psycholinguistic background while filling the gaps and making inferences to make sense of what they are exposed to (Anastasiou & Griva, Citation2009). Regardless of the theoretical position taken, there is consensus among teachers and educators that academic and professional success entails a developed reading comprehension skill since attaining expertise in any field relies on the individuals’ ability to digest a large body of written input that is mostly presented in English textbooks and journals (Zare, Citation2007).

An intrinsic property of reading comprehension is that it is not an acquired skill and entails instruction. Based on this conviction, teachers and researchers have long been seeking to cultivate the most effective instructional approach in teaching reading. Yet, the theoretical active/passive divide in conceptualization of reading is also evident in the way it is taught in practice. Owing to the still prevailing misconception of reading comprehension as a language learning activity, some teachers eschew the critical potential of reading comprehension in favor of lexical and grammatical development. Based on the common experience of English teachers, most of the difficulties that plethora of EFL learners, including Iranian learners, face in comprehending written texts seem to derive from this widespread illusion the product of which is a highly restricted skill that hardly enables the learners to read between the lines.

The proponents of recent communicative and critical stand, on the other hand, accentuate comprehension as an initial stage in critical thinking (CT) and emphasize the need to engage readers in critically analyzing, synthesizing and evaluating the quality of what they read (Jun, Citation2011). Theoretical support for this critical interpretation is offered by Bloom’s hierarchical taxonomy for CT (1956) which starts with comprehension as the bottom layer and analysis, synthesis and evaluation as the higher levels of thinking. Research findings in applied linguistics highlight the compatibility of CT in teaching of reading comprehension and link EFL learners’ problems in reading to exclusive attempt to improve learners’ comprehension through lexical and grammatical knowledge with no parallel attempt to encourage the development of higher-order thinking skills (Pressley, Citation2002) and reasoning skills (Van Gelder, Citation2005).The burgeoning research in CT since the 1990s has unravelled the paramount importance of developing learners’ comprehension along with both their CT skills and metacognitive strategies (MCSs).

The concomitant instruction of CT along with instruction in basic skills such as reading comprehension represent the infusion approach to CT that has been advocated by numerous experts like Bailin, Case, Coombs, and Daniels (Citation1999). Educationalists view CT as a domain-specific potential capacity that entails both in-depth instruction in the subject matter and explicit instruction on general principles of CT. They justify the teachability of CT on grounds that learners can be trained to analyze arguments and claims via making inferences, inductive and deductive reasoning, analyzing and synthesizing and evaluating the input before arriving at decisions and solutions (Facione, Citation1990). Willingham (Citation2007) delineated the CT awareness-raising activities that can naturally occur in a reading classroom including activating background knowledge, asking and answering questions, defining terms, identifying assumptions, interpreting and explaining meaning, verbal reasoning in relation to likelihood and uncertainty, predicting and seeing both sides of an issue. As such, CT skills may be drawn from the content to be learnt through the natural consequence of engaging with the subject matter (Ennis, Citation1989). It is argued that learners can thus function more efficiently by evaluating the validity of the information they receive, making more accurate decisions and learning how to learn and what to believe (Braun, Citation2004).

CT skills start with comprehension of the written input which may still pose serious problems for numerous EFL learners. One way out of this predicament, according to research findings (Oxford, Citation1990; Tavakoli, Citation2014), is raising learners’ awareness of metacognitive strategies (MCSs) that indicate the ways readers interact with written texts (Erfanpour, Citation2013) and are congruent with learner autonomy as the ultimate goal in learning-centered instruction. Operationalization of metacognitive awareness-raising by Mokhtari and Reichard (Citation2002) in the form of Metacognitive Awareness of Reading Strategy Inventory (MARSI) allowed practitioners to measure the extent to which adult readers employ strategies to comprehend what they read. This framework can also be employed to train learners to promote their comprehension of the text, which is the initial stage of CT, through strategic reading. Hence, metacognitive training can facilitate comprehension whereas CT awareness-raising can boost critical analysis and evaluation of the input. The extent to which application of these two methodological alternatives may contribute to overall comprehension across various genres is still unexplored.

A glance through the related literature indicates that despite a plethora of research dealing with MCSs, CT and reading comprehension distinctively (Erfanpour, Citation2013; Fahim & Kamali, Citation2011; Ismail & Tawalbeh, Citation2015),literature is not so rich with respect to the extent to which metacognitive strategy training or CT-awareness raising may contribute to EFL learners’ reading comprehension of various textual genres particularly general, argumentative, and cause of effect texts. Therefore, the current inquiry set out to explore the influence of these two methodological options, as the independent variables, on Iranian EFL learners’ comprehension of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts. It was assumed that exploring the effectiveness of these two instructional interventions across textual genres would not only provide insight on their effectiveness but also narrow down teachers’ choices by transparently delineating pedagogical options based on learners’ needs and teaching content. To serve the purpose, the following research questions were formulated:

  1. Does metacognitive strategy training have any significant differential impact on EFL learners’ reading comprehension of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts?

  2. Does CT awareness-raising have any significant differential impact on EFL learners’ reading comprehension of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts?

  3. Do metacognitive strategy training and CT awareness-raising have any significant differential impact on EFL learners’ reading comprehension of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts?

  4. Is there any significant difference in the three groups’ performance on the general, cause and effect, and argumentative reading comprehension post-test?

2. Theoretical context and literature review

2.1. MCSs, and reading comprehension

Knowledge of metacognition in reading incorporates one’s familiarity with the reason and the objective of reading (Eskey, Citation2005) as well as the way comprehension problems could be tackled. Metacognitive system learning appears to be basic if readers are to successfully control their methodology use while reading (Phakiti, Citation2003a). One of the principal meanings of metacognition originates from Flavell (Citation1976), who described it as one’s learning concerning one’s own subjective procedures and items or anything identified with them. Yet, there is no regular comprehension of the connection between student utilization of learning strategies and learning achievement, or whether and how system use is reflected in test outcomes (Saks & Leigen, Citation2018). Indeed, metacognition, as stated by Mokhtari and Reichard (Citation2002), has been characterized as the mindfulness and observing procedures that comprise the information of the readers’ discernment about perusing and the restraint components they practice when monitoring and managing content perception.

Numerous studies have addressed the relationship between reading comprehension, metacognition, and CT. For instance, Phakiti (Citation2003b) examined the relationship between 384 Thai learners’ cognitive and metacognitive strategy use and their reading test performance through the use of a cognitive and metacognitive questionnaire, retrospective interviews and an EFL achievement test. Research is also replete with studies addressing the impact of various metacognitive training programs on listening comprehension (Coskun, Citation2010), and reading comprehension (Hong-Nam & Leavel, Citation2011), EFL and ESL reading comprehension (Aghaie & Zhang, Citation2012; Estacio, Citation2013; Iwai, Citation2011; Jafari & Ketabi, Citation2012; Karbalaei, Citation2011; Seifoori, Citation2018).The findings suggest that explicit strategy training can improve the participants’ reading and listening comprehension as well as their autonomous reading behaviors (Aghaie & Zhang, Citation2012). Metacognitive training was also found effective in enhancing less proficient and low accomplishing students’ learning (Ismail & Tawalbeh, Citation2015). In another study, Riyadi, Hersulastuti, and Nugrahaningsih (Citation2017) developed a theoretical model of reading comprehension based on MCSs and found underlining, note-taking, abridging, and idea mapping as significantly enhancing senior secondary school students’ comprehension. More recently, Shih and Huang (Citation2018) underscored the number and variety of MCSs used by the students as major determinants of reading performance during reading testing situations.

2.2. CT and reading comprehension

Comprehension, as already stated, is only the first stage of CT that can provoke profound thinking procedures and culminate in reconfiguration of one’s conceptual system. That is why Beck (Citation1999) rules out the viability of reading without reasoning. Yet, for such thought-provoking reading comprehension to take place, it is vital to raise learners’ awareness of CT skills which incorporate the cognitive abilities of thinking, making a deduction, analyzing the arguments, settling on choices and assessing the validity of the information. As such, reading comprehension might be envisaged as closely relevant to principles of CT, as suggested by Cohen (Citation2004).

Research findings have substantiated the positive effectiveness of CT in boosting reading comprehension (Barjesteh & Vaseghi., Citation2012; Gomez, Citation2010; Mendenhall & Johnson, Citation2010). Fahim and Kamali (Citation2011) attributed learners’ ability to find unknown words in a reading comprehension text to their CT skills. In another study, Fahim and Sa’eepour (Citation2011) explored the effect of training CT aptitudes on reading comprehension and found that CT aptitudes prompted students’ reading comprehension. CT skills were also found to be significantly and strongly correlated with Iranian EFL learners’ reading comprehension (GhorbanDordiNejad & Heydari, Citation2012) and university students’ self-efficacy (Dehghani, Jafarisani, Pakmehr, & Malekzadeh, Citation2011).

All the above-mentioned and many other empirical studies have explored reading comprehension either in relation to CT or MCSs while others have detected the influence of each of these variables on participants’ reading comprehension. The present study, however, is distinct from what has been undertaken so far for two reasons. First, it acknowledged the complementary nature of comprehension and CT, on the one hand, and the contributions that metacognitive training can make to promote comprehension, on the other. Thus, the study took a comparative stand and investigated the differential effects of each of these interventions on the participants’ reading comprehension.

Secondly, reading comprehension was operationalized as closely related to generic structure of the text since Iranian EFL learners seem to perform differently while reading various textual genres. Iranian students seem to have fewer problems in deciphering meaning while reading general texts compared to cause and effect and argumentative texts. CT includes the constituent skills of analyzing arguments, making inferences, using inductive or deductive reasoning, evaluating and making decisions. By the same token, cause and effect and argumentative input seem to comprise embedded intentions, implied meanings and covert premises comprehension of which entails both a set of metacognitive strategies and CT skills. It is, nevertheless, doubtful whether comprehension problems with such texts have their roots in the learners’ unawareness of CR skills or underdeveloped metacognitive strategies.

According to Van Gelder (Citation2005) and Willingham (Citation2007), metacognition appears to be subsumed under CT because CT entails the ability to deploy the right strategies and skills at the right time and this ability is typically referred to as conditional or strategic knowledge which is part of metacognition. Similarly, Halpern (Citation1998) conceive of metacognition as monitoring thinking and strategy use by asking questions like: What do I already know? What is my goal? How will I know when I get there? Am I making progress? Even though MCSs are considered as subcategories of CT, both consist of similar and non-similar subcategories. That is to say, MCSs focused in the present study tended to promote learners’ initial comprehension of the three types of texts while the CT skills selected aimed at enabling the participants to analyze, synthesize and evaluate the target texts. The rationale for taking this comparative approach was to add to the existing literature by detecting more precisely the extent to which each of these two methodological manipulations can maximize comprehension of general, argumentative and cause and effect texts.

3. Methodology

3.1. Participants

A purposeful sample of 54 intermediate male Iranian EFL learners attending three classes in an English Language Institute in Qazvin, Iran, were selected from a pool of 85 English learners based on their performance on a Preliminary English Test (PET) that was run to verify the groups’ homogeneity in reading comprehension. Those whose scores fell within two standard deviation above and below the mean were selected as the research sample. The participants were speakers of Persian and were learning English as a second language. The three groups, each including 18 participants, were randomly assigned as two experimental groups, receiving metacognitive training (MCG) and CT awareness-raising (CTG), and one control group(CG) that was instructed based on the conventional pre-view, view, post-view model of teaching reading with no reference to metacognitive strategies nor CT skills.

3.2. Instruments

To probe the research questions, we made use of a PET and a researcher-made reading comprehension test.

3.2.1. Preliminary English test (PET)

PET is a standardized proficiency test developed for intermediate learners. The original test comprises different subtests; Yet, in line with the focus of the study, we employed the reading comprehension subtest to check the homogeneity of the participants at the onset of the study. It comprised of five reading comprehension texts and 35 comprehension questions. The texts were followed by multiple-choice, matching items, true/false multiple-choice cloze items.

3.2.2. Reading comprehension test

The study was concerned with the participants’ comprehension of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts. To this end, we developed a reading comprehension test including three general, three argumentative and three cause and effect texts selected from sample TOEFL reading texts and each followed by six multiple-choice items. To answer the questions correctly, as is the norm with TOEFL, participants had to employ a wide range of strategies and CT skills to analyze and judge the information to obtain the total test score of 54. The test was piloted with a group sharing the characteristics of the norm group for whom it had been constructed. The reliability coefficient was found to be .81 on KR21.

Further, the actual test was administered as the pre-test and the post-test under identical conditions with a change in the order of the reading texts, the items and the options. The time allotted to each of the tests was 90 minutes.

3.2.3. The MARSI

The MARSI, developed by Mokhtari and Reichard (Citation2002), was employed as the basic framework for the metacognitive training program. It comprises three subcomponent strategy types. Global reading strategies (GRSs)with 13 items (1,3,4,7,10,14,17,19,22,23,25,26,29) that represent generalized or global reading strategies aimed at setting the stage for the reading act, for instance, setting a purpose for reading and previewing text content. Problem-solving reading strategies (PSSs)include 8 items (8,11,13,16,18,21,27,30) that address localized focused problem solving or repair strategies such as checking understanding which are employed when information processing becomes problematic. Finally, the support reading strategy (SRSs)subcomponent consists of 9 items (2, 5,6,9,12,15,20,24,28) that check the reader’s use of support mechanisms and tools like reference materials aimed at sustaining responsiveness to reading.

3.3. Procedures

Having selected the research sample including three groups of participants in three intact classes, the study began with the administration of the reading pre-test to check whether the participants were homogeneous in their comprehension of general, argumentative and cause and effect text types.

Next, the treatment in both the MCG and the CTG and the placebo in the CG started and continued for 36 sessions over a time span of two semesters, 18 sessions each. Each session lasted for 90 minutes during which the participants worked on two reading texts and were assigned a third text as homework. The texts were selected from authentic reading comprehension textbooks. The content included a proportionate number of general, cause and effect and argumentative texts. The groups were taught the same content through different procedures. Each semester, the first two sessions were devoted to introduction of the course and pre-testing the participants’ reading comprehension. The last session was also devoted to the post-tests. Thus, the treatment lasted for 30 sessions.

3.3.1. The MCG

The training program designed for the MCG was based on Metacognitive Awareness of Reading Strategies Inventory (MARSI) which is a 30-item and 5-point Likert-type scale that rates adolescent and adult language learners’ perceived use of reading strategies. In the present study, however, it was employed as a pedagogical framework to present metacognitive strategies. The MCG received training based on the CALLA model (Chamot, Barnhardt, El-Dinary, & Robbins, Citation1999; Chamot & O’Malley, Citation1994). The model was employed through five basic steps including “preparation, presentation, practice, evaluation, and expansion” (Oxford, Citation1990).

During the preparation stage, which lasted for about five minutes, the instructor asked some general pre-reading questions related to the topic to activate the participants’ background knowledge. In the next 20 minutes, which was the presentation stage and would normally take approximately 20 minutes of the class time, two of MCSs were presented by asking the participants some general questions such as “what do you do if you do not understand the meaning of a word while reading?”; they were directed at the target strategies. Further, the operations to tackle those problems were explicitly and interactively presented followed by examples to clarify the concepts. The next thing to do was to consolidate the presented strategies through practice. Hence, a reading text was assigned to be read individually in approximately 15 minutes. To answer the comprehension questions following the text required application of the strategies they had been taught. During the practice stage, the teacher was sitting at her desk not to disturb the participants’ concentration. Then, they were given a few minutes to check their responses in pairs before giving feedback to the class. The feedback phase would take approximately 10 minutes.

Having comprehended the assigned text, the participants were invited to evaluate their application of the strategies and the usefulness of each in enhancing comprehension in10 minutes. Meanwhile, they were encouraged to compare their prior experiences of reading comprehension with what they had just done in class and to realize the difference strategic reading can make to comprehension. Further, the teacher would emphasize the necessity of expanding strategic reading to outside classroom situations by applying the same strategies while reading different texts and for a wide range of purposes. Each session culminated in a supplementary reading comprehension task that was either a cause and effect or an argumentative in nature. Their responses were reviewed in the class and the problems were resolved interactively. Finally, a third text was assigned as homework.

3.3.2. The CTG

Following the instructional method offered by Jun (Citation2011) and insights from Paul et al., (1997), the instruction in the CTG aimed at helping learners to develop four mental abilities of interpreting, analyzing, evaluating and explaining written input. The focus of awareness-raising in this group was on major concepts of CT like detecting clues and interpreting content, analyzing the text to identify intended meaning and actual inferential relations among statements to draw reasonable conclusions, evaluating the validity of content by assessing the credibility of statements and finally presenting the results of personal reasoning in the form of a coherent and cogent explanation. These four CT skills were initially presented in the first session of each semester using PowerPoint presentation and examples to clarify each skill. The instructional sessions were focused on four-stage interpretation, analysis, evaluation and explanation of the course content, each lasting for 20 minutes.

For each instructional session, teaching of reading started with the first phase of the training which was analysis. The teacher introduced the reading comprehension topic asking the participants to discuss it and state what they expected to find in the text. Then, they were required to skim the text to see how it conformed to their expectations. The focus in this stage was on engaging the participants in getting an overall interpretation of the text. Participants interpretations were then heard without any feedback.

The second phase of teaching was focused on text analysis which was a level higher than interpretation. It involved identifying the meaning of unknown words based on the context clues, intended meaning and actual inferential relations among statements, questions or descriptions that aimed to express belief, judgements and reasons. The participants were taught how to find out the writer’s main purpose and point of view regarding the topic and the main questions reflected in the text.

Evaluating the logic of the passage was carried out during the third phase of teaching process when the participants read the text line by line and judge whether the writer could clearly state the main idea and achieve the goal of elaborating it accurately and adequately. Finally, they were required to explain their evaluation of the text in the form of an evaluative summary.

In the following 15 minutes of the class time, the teacher would assign a reading comprehension task followed by questions that required the participants to interpret, analyze, evaluate and explain the text as homework. The three texts represented general, cause and effect and argumentative texts ordered based on the course content and the difficulty level of the texts.

3.4. Data analysis

The collected data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 20). Having checked the normality of the data and the homogeneity of the three groups via ANOVA analysis of the pre-test scores, the experimental groups’ pre-test and post-test scores were compared through paired-samples t-tests to answer research questions one and two. The third research question was answered using three independent samples t-tests to compare the participants’ post-test scores to compare the effectiveness of the two interventions. Finally, the three groups’ comprehension of general, argumentative and cause and effect texts were compared via one-way between groups ANOVA analysis.

4. Results and discussion

4.1. The pre-test scores

Prior to any statistical analyses, the Shapiro-Wilk (SW) test was run on the pre-test scores to check the normality assumption. The results are depicted in Table .

Table 1. Tests of normality of the groups’ pre-test scores

As shown in Table , the results of the SW, used for samples smaller than 2000, show that all the scores were normally distributed, (p > .001). Further, the groups’ pre-test scores were compared via a One-Way ANOVA to ensure their initial homogeneity in reading comprehension. Table presents the results.

Table 2. ANOVA for the groups’ pre-test scores

A glance at Table indicates that the observed differences in the groups’ scores did not reach significance level, p > .05, thus, confirming the initial homogeneity of the groups in reading comprehension.

4.2. The post-test scores

It was essential to check the normality of the post-test scores through another Shapiro Wilks test prior to analyzing the data to answer the research questions. The results are depicted in Table .

Table 3. Tests of normality for the groups’ post-test scores

Based on the results in Table , all the score sets were normally distributed with regard to the SW test, (p > .001).

4.3. The effect of MCG on GR, CER, and ARGR

The group’s descriptive statistics were calculated, as illustrated in Table , and revealed some differences.

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for the MCG and CTG’s pre/post-test scores

The results depicted increases in the group’s scores from the pre-test to the post-test in GR (Pre-test M = 8.94 to Post-test M = 10.72) and in CER (Pre-test M = 9.56 to Post-test M = 10.05) and in ARGR from (M = 8.83) to (M = 10.50).

The first research question addressed the effect of MCG on the participants’ GR, CER, and ARGR. This question was answered through a paired samples t-test analysis of the data. Table presents the results.

Table 5. Paired samples t-test for the MCG’s pre-test/post-test scores

As displayed in Table , the magnitude of the differences in the group’s mean scores was significant for GR, t (34) = −2.13, p = .04, two-tailed, and ARG, t (34) = −3.22, p = .001, two-tail. However, the difference in CER scores did not reach significance level, t (34) = −.90, p = .38, two-tailed.

4.4. The effect of CT awareness-raising on GR, CER, and ARGR

To test the effect of CRAR on the participants’ GR, CER, and ARGR, as posed in the second research question, the descriptive statistics of the pre-test and post-test scores sets were calculated, as presented in Table , and the significance of the differences in the scores was tested via paired samples t-test, the results of which are revealed in Table .

Table 6. Paired samples t-test for the CTG’s pre-test/post-test scores

As Table indicates, the differences from the pre-test to the post-test were significant in GR scores, t(34) = −3.98, p = .001, two-tailed, and ARGR scores, t (34) = 0.2.68, p = 0.02, two-tailed; the magnitude of the differences in the means was large for GR (.53) and ARGR (.25) (Cohen Citation2004). The difference in the CER scores, yet, did not reach significance level, t (34) = −1.21, p = 0.24, two-tailed.

4.5. The comparative effect of MCG and CTG on GR, CE, and ARG scores

The third research question delved into the comparative effect of the two independent variables on the participants’ reading comprehension of the three genres. To answer this research question, hence, the groups’ post-test general, cause and effect, and argumentative reading comprehension scores were analyzed through independent samples t-tests. Tables , , and 9 display the results of the independent samples t-tests respectively.

Table 7. Independent-samples t-test for the MCG and CTG’s Post-test GR scores

Table 8. Independent-samples t-test for the MCG and CTG’s post-test CER scores

According to Table , there was no significant difference in the GR scores for the MCG (M = 10.72, SD = 1.93) and the CTG (M = 10.22, SD = 1.51), t(34) = .86, p = 0.39, two-tailed; the magnitude of the differences in the means (mean difference = .57, 95% CI: −6.7 to 1.67) was small (eta squared = .02) (Cohen, Citation2004).

According to Table , there was no significant difference was observed in the CER scores of the MCG (M = 10.05, SD = 1.61) and the CTG (M = 9.00., SD = 1.87), t (34) = 1.48, p = 0.08, two-tailed. The magnitude of the differences in the means (mean difference = 1.05, 95% CI:—1.40 to 2.25) was small (eta squared = .08) (Cohen, Citation2004).

According to Table , there was no significant difference between the ARG scores of the MCG (M = 10.50, SD = 2.00) and the CTG(M = 9.94, SD = 2.18), t (34) = 0.79, p = 0.43, two-tailed; the magnitude of the differences in the means (mean difference = .55, 95% CI:—8.64 to 1.97) was small (eta squared = .019) (Cohen, Citation2004).

Table 9. Independent-samples t-test for the MCG and CTG ‘s Post-test ARGR scores

4.6. Comparing the three groups’ reading post-test scores

The last research question addressed the comparative effect of the three methodological interventions, MC training, CT awareness-raising and conventional teaching of the content, on the participants’ reading comprehension post-test scores to decide whether the observed enhancement in the two experimental groups could be attributed to the treatments or the course of instruction they underwent. To serve the purpose, three one-way between-groups ANOVA tests were run on the groups’ GR, CER, and ARGR, as measured by the reading comprehension post-test. The results are presented in Table .

Table 10. One-way between-groups ANOVA test for the reading post-test scores

As displayed in Table , there was a statistically significant difference at the p < .05 level for the three groups in GR scores: F (2, 51) = 5.61, p. = 0.006,CER scores: F (2, 51) = 3.49, p. = 0.038, and ARGR scores: F (2, 51) = 6.14, p. = 0.004. The effect size, calculated using eta squared, was 0.18, 0.12, and 0.19 for the GR, CE, and ARG reading, respectively, all regarded as medium (Cohen, Citation2004). To locate the difference more specifically, a Tukey HSD test was run, the results of which are presented in Table .

Table 11. Between groups’ comparison Tukey HSD test

The Post-hoc comparison, as illustrated in Table , indicated that the MCG and the CTG significantly outperformed the CG in GR and ARGR. With respect to cause and effect texts, however, the MCG could significantly outperform the CG.

4.7. Discussion

The findings emerging from the present enquiry indicated that CT awareness-raising and MC training could boost the participants’ reading comprehension of general and argumentative texts and that CT failed to enhance their comprehension of texts expressing causal relationship. As far as the effect of metacognitive training is concerned, the outcomes of the current investigation are in conformity with some previous inquiries (Aghaie & Zhang, Citation2012; Akkakoson & Setobol, Citation2009; Estacio, Citation2013; Ismail & Tawalbeh, Citation2015; Jafari & Ketabi, Citation2012; Karbalaei, Citation2011; Mehrpour, Sadighi, & Bagheri, Citation2012; Riyadi et al., Citation2017; Shih & Huang, Citation2018) that confirmed facilitative effects of MC training on EFL learners’ reading comprehension.

The findings are, nonetheless, incompatible with those of Mante (Citation2009), Korotaeva (Citation2012) and Mehrdad, Ahghar, and Ahghar (Citation2012) who found no significant effect from metacognitive training onEFL learners’ reading comprehension, respectively. The findings also run counter to those reported by Pammu, Amir, and Maasum (Citation2014) who found use of various MCSs ineffective in improving Indonesian EFL learners’ comprehension. The investigations about the instruction of metacognitive reading strategies are still at an exploratory stage and further empirical and theoretical inspections need to be carried out.

The contradictory research findings regarding the effect of MC training between this and previous studies might be explained from the perspective of Adaptive Control of Thought (ACT) Model (Anderson, Citation1983) with regard to the degree of proceduralization of the strategic knowledge achieved. ACT postulates a declarative/procedural continuum with various forms of knowledge at either poles and learning is defined as the Proceduralization of knowledge through practice. In other words, explicit learning typical of classroom contexts demands focal attention while processing information during the cognitive stage of skill development. It is subsequent meaningful practice that can trigger more neural associations and culminate in proceduralization of knowledge by releasing attentional load (Anderson, Citation1983). Strategy training, like any other kind of learning, is assumed to be governed by the same principles. Participants in the MCG seem to have succeeded in making the shift from cognitive to associative and autonomy stages in their strategic knowledge while reading general and argumentative texts in and outside the class.

The improvement of the MCG could also be explicated in terms of input processing instruction (VanPatten, Citation1996) that acknowledges the difficulty of concurrently attending to different stimuli in the input and postulates that learners need to prioritize their attentional resources while receiving and processing input. This model puts forward a number of default processing strategies such as processing lexical and grammatical items. The instructional aim within IPI is to help learners overcome these default processing tendencies via explicit explanation of the target structures, explicit strategy training to enable learners to overcome the default processing strategy, and structured input activities. Although the participant in the MCG relied more heavily on the second technique and received explicit strategy training in using three sets of metacognitive reading strategies which seems to have helped them noticed the organizational and structural features of all texts to overcome their default processing tendencies.

Moreover, the finding corroborates theoretical postulations and empirical evidence about the effectiveness of metacognitive instruction in enhancing reading comprehension. It is specifically documented in the literature that metacognitive training facilitates processing and storing of information (Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, Citation2010), helps readers to manage cognitive processes and difficulties while reading (Goh, Citation2008), and directs them to self-appraising and self-regulating of their learning (Wenden, Citation1998). In this way, learners understand the active nature of reading process and the need for particular effort from their part (Goh, Citation2008) to use certain strategies to enhance their understanding.

As for the CTG, the findings substantiate the effectiveness of CT awareness-raising in enhancing EFL learners’ analytic, evaluative and explanatory skills which, according to Fahim and Kamali (Citation2011) can develop the competency to reason. The methodology used in presenting and practicing CT skills in the current study followed the same explicit/implicit and declarative/procedural route as the metacognitive reading strategies. Hence, the findings might be explained with regard to the proceduralization of CT skills (Anderson, Citation1983). Application of these CT skills, as suggested by Gomez (Citation2010), involves reflecting on the legitimacy of what learners read in light of their earlier information. The improvement in the CTAR group could also be corroborated with regard to the consolidation of CT awareness-raising with the reading comprehension course since, as posited by Silva (Citation2008), knowledge and thinking have to be taught simultaneously and CT provides a vehicle for teaching reading comprehension skills. The outcomes of the current investigation are in accordance with those of Abrami et al. (Citation2008), Fahim and Sa’eepour (Citation2011) and Barjesteh and Vaseghi. (Citation2012) who found CT training effective in enhancing EFL learners’ reading comprehension skill.

Further, the findings revealed no significant difference between MCG and CTG in comprehension of the three reading genres. A few studies, however, found MC and CT unrelated (e.g. Kamid, Citation2013) and a few experts (McPeck, Citation1990) argue that CT and metacognition are distinct constructs. In contrast with those findings, the results from the present research lend support to the similarity of metacognitive and critical thinking skills and accentuate the possibility of developing reading comprehension skills through both types of training. This finding is consistent with previous studies (Anderson, Citation2002; Choy & Cheah, Citation2009; Daud & Hafsari, Citation2015; Kaur, Citation2017; Ku & Ho, Citation2010; Kuhn & Dean, Citation2004; Magno, Citation2010; Mall-Amiri & Ahmadi, Citation2014; Nemat-Tabrizi & Erfani, Citation2014).

Surprisingly, neither of the instructional interventions could promote the participants’ comprehension of causality from the pre-test to the post-test viably because of the complexity and diversity of distinctive structural and organizational features expressing causality, the participants’ inadequate processing mechanisms to differentiate those features and lack of pedagogical focus on such features in the program. In fact, what characterizes cause and effect structure, according to McCrudden et al., (Citation2007) is a number of distinctive characteristics that render it one of the most difficult organizational patterns to comprehend. At the simplest level, cause and effect structure presents a single cause linked to a single effect. Yet, the top-level arrangement of causes and effects may vary widely and thus pose a challenge in detecting and comprehending the causal relationship partly due to the limits of short-term memory. The highest levels of incomprehensibility are associated with texts in which the causal consequent is not directly connected to its antecedent. As Linderholm et al. (Citation2000) suggested, such texts oblige the reader to infer the cause, or retrieve information from personal background knowledge, each of which may expectedly hamper comprehension of less-skilled readers who are not proficient enough to predict cause and effect relationships. In fact, these features were beyond the reach of the interventions and remained untouched from the pre-test to the post-test. The only significant improvement was observed in the MCG in contrast to the CG. The superiority of the MCG over the CG can indicate the difference metacognitive strategy use can make to overcome their default processing tendencies.MC training seems to be a more promising alternative to conventional methods of teaching reading.

5. Conclusion

The current study suffered from a few limitations and delimitations including the sample size and purposeful sampling procedure, disregard of learner variables like age, gender and intellectual propensities as well as restricting the study to reading comprehension and constraining data collection instruments to a single test. Due to these restrictions and the contradictory results reported by other researchers (Mante, Citation2009 & Korotaeva, Citation2012), it may be premature to postulate a definite causal association between MC and CT training and reading comprehension. More studies have to be conducted with larger samples of male and female EFL learners at varying levels of proficiency, age and representing various individual characteristics before arriving at more definite conclusions.

Nevertheless, the findings underscore the necessity of realigning the conventional methods of teaching reading by incorporating methodological procedures that provoke metacognitive reading strategies or higher order thinking operations of analysis, synthesis and evaluative explanation. This basic requirement is justifiable with respect to the growing demand for reading comprehension skills to deal with incessant flow of intricate and conflicting information targeting the general public. They need to be trained how to. MC training and CT awareness-raising, as suggested by the findings, can help individuals to process the information adequately and precisely enough to arrive ate sound and right judgements.

Secondly, the findings underscore the EFL learners’ need for expanding their organizational competence regarding various writing genres and particularly the cause and effect organizational signals in learning English as a foreign language. The findings unraveled the multifaceted nature of teaching reading and the multiplicity of the factors like textual genre that need to be taken into account to promote EFL leaners’ comprehension. This need might be satisfied at a small-scale by individual teachers who select to broaden and deepen their repertoire of strategy-based instruction and CT skills and incorporate MCSs and CT skills into their teaching (Vandergrift, Citation1999) in order to develop their students’ strategic competence and thinking skills. EFL teachers are, thus, invited to reconsider the way they teach reading and see how they may accommodate learner development through strategic and CT investment. Alternatively, the demand might be met at a large-scale through teacher education and bottom-up curriculum development by obtaining insights from practicing teachers and teacher educators regarding comprehension problems students face, and thereby, designing teacher training course focused on strategic reading and the role of CT skills to develop the teachers’ strategic teaching skills as a first step in promoting reading comprehension at a national scale. In addition, teacher educators should familiarize student teachers with theoretical issues and empirical findings regarding MCSs and CT in pre-service courses. Last, but not least, individual EFL learners are recommended to take the lead in their own learning by expanding their strategic competence and CT skills through self-development.

Additional information

Funding

The authors received no direct funding for this research.

Notes on contributors

Fatemeh Mohseni

Fatemeh Mohseni is a PhD candidate in Azad University of Tabriz, Iran. She teaches some teaching courses in Azad University of Tabriz, Iran. She has published some articles in EFL field.

Zohreh Seifoori

Zohreh Seifoori has a PhD in TEFL. She is an associate professor and has been teaching for 22 years at Islamic Azad University, Tabriz branch. She has published and presented a number of papers in different international journals and conferences. Her main interests are alternative assessment, teacher education, and teaching strategies.

Saeideh Ahangari

Saeideh Ahangari is an assistant professor in TEFL at Islamic Azad University, Tabriz Branch. She has published a number of papers and participated in many national and international conferences. Her main interests are task-based language teaching, CALL and their interface with the issues in language testing.

References

  • Abrami, P. C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Surkes, M. A., Tamim, R., & Zhang, D. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills and dispositions: A stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 1102–22. doi:10.3102/0034654308326084
  • Aghaie, R., & Zhang, L. J. (2012). Effects of explicit instruction in cognitive and metacognitive reading strategies on Iranian EFL students’ reading performance and strategy transfer. Instructional Science, 40(6), 1063–1081. doi:10.1007/s11251-011-9202-5
  • Akkakoson, S., & Setobol, B. (2009). Thai EFL use of strategies in reading English texts. The Journal of Kmutnb, 19(3), 125–140.
  • Anastasiou, D., & Griva, E. (2009). Awareness of reading strategy use and reading comprehension among poor readers. Elementary Education Online, 8(2), 281–297.
  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Anderson, N. J. (2002). The role of metacognitive in second language teaching and learning. Modern Language Journal, 75(4), 460–472. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4781.1991.tb05384.x
  • Bailin, S., Case, R., Coombs, J. R., & Daniels, L. B. (1999). Conceptualizing critical thinking. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31(3), 285–302. doi:10.1080/002202799183133
  • Barjesteh, H., & Vaseghi., R. (2012). Critical thinking: A reading strategy in developing English reading comprehension performance. Sheikhbahaee EFL Journal, 1(1), 76–85.
  • Beck, I. L. (1999). Reading and Reasoning. The Reading Teacher, 42(6), 676–682.
  • Braun, N. M. (2004). Critical thinking in the business curriculum. Journal of Education for Business, 79(1), 232–236.
  • Chamot, A. U., Barnhardt, S., El-Dinary, P., & Robbins, J. (1999). The learning strategies handbook. New York: Addison Wesley Longman.
  • Chamot, A. U., & O’Malley, J. (1994). The CALLA handbook: Implementing the cognitive academic language learning approach. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley.
  • Choy, S. C., & Cheah, P. K. (2009). Teacher perceptions of critical thinking among students and its influence on higher education. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 20(2), 198–206.
  • Cohen, A. D. (2004). Strategies in learning and using a second language. New York, NY: Addison-Wesley.
  • Coskun, A. (2010). The effect of metacognitive strategy training on the listening performance of beginner students. Novitas-ROYAL (Research on Youth and Language), 4(1), 35–50.
  • Daud, F., & Hafsari, I. (2015). The contribution of critical thinking skills and metacognitive awareness on students’ learning: Teaching biology at senior high school. Modern Applied Science, 9(12), 143–153. doi:10.5539/mas.v9n12p143
  • Dehghani, M., Jafarisani, H., Pakmehr, H., & Malekzadeh, A. (2011). Relationship between students’ critical thinking and self-efficacy beliefs in Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Iran. Journal of Social and Behavioral Sciences, 15(3), 2952–2955.
  • Ennis, R. H. (1989). Critical thinking and subject specificity: Clarification and needed research. Educational Researcher, 18(3), 4–10. doi:10.3102/0013189X018003004
  • Erfanpour, M. A. (2013). The effect of intensive and extensive reading strategies on reading comprehension: A case of Iranian high school students. English for Specific Purposes World, 14(6), 1–7.
  • Eskey, D. E. (2005). Reading in a second language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Estacio, M. J. M. (2013). Bilingual readers’ metacognitive strategies as predictors of reading comprehension. Philippine ESL Journal, 10(3), 179–199.
  • Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: A statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction(The Delphi Report). The California Academic Press.
  • Fahim, M., & Kamali, Z. (2011). The relationship between critical thinking ability of Iranian EFL learners and their resilience level facing unfamiliar vocabulary items in reading. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 2(1), 101–114.
  • Fahim, M., & Sa’eepour, M. (2011). The impact of teaching critical thinking skills on reading comprehension of Iranian EFL learners. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 2(4), 867–874. doi:10.4304/jltr.2.4.867-874
  • Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. The Nature of Intelligence, 12(2), 231–235.
  • GhorbanDordiNejad, F., & Heydari, M. (2012). On the relationship between Iranian EFL students’ critical thinking ability and their reading comprehension micro-skills. Philippine ESL Journal, 9(3), 130–141.
  • Goh, C. (2008). Metacognitive instruction for second Language listening development: Theory, practice and research implications. RELC Journal, 39(2), 188–213. doi:10.1177/0033688208092184
  • Gomez, J. C. (2010). The impact of structural reading lessons on the development of critical thinking skills. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 7(1), 32–48.
  • Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53(4), 449–455. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449
  • Hong-Nam, K., & Leavel, A. G. (2011). Reading strategy instruction, metacognitive awareness, and self-perception of striving college developmental readers. Journal of College Literature and Learning, 37(3), 3–17.
  • Ismail, N. M., & Tawalbeh, T. I. (2015). Effectiveness of a metacognitive reading strategies program for improving low achieving EFL readers. International Education Studies, 8(1), 71–81.
  • Iwai, Y. (2011). Metacognitive awareness and strategy use in academic English reading among adult English as a Second Language (ESL) students. ProQuest, UMI Dissertation Publishing.
  • Jafari, D., & Ketabi, S. (2012). Metacognitive strategies and reading comprehension enhancement in Iranian intermediate EFL setting. International Journal of Linguistics, 4(3), 1–14. doi:10.5296/ijl.v4i3.1684
  • Jun, X. (2011). The application of critical thinking in teaching English reading. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 1(2), 136–141.
  • Kamid. (2013). Students’ metacognition in solving mathematical problems Case study on junior high school students based on gender. Edumatica, 3(1), 64–72.
  • Karbalaei, A. (2011). Assessing reading strategy training based on CALLA model in EFL and ESL context. Íkala, Revista De Lenguaje y Cultura, 16(27), 167–187.
  • Kaur, K. (2017). Influence of demographic factors on metacognition and its relationship with critical thinking of higher secondary students: Foundation for learning. International Journal for Research in Applied Science & Engineering Technology (IJRASET), 5(7), 358–363.
  • Korotaeva, I. V. (2012). Metacognitive strategies in reading comprehension of education majors. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 69(5), 1895–1900. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.143
  • Ku, K. Y. L., & Ho, I. T. (2010). Metacognitive strategies that enhance critical thinking. Metacognition and Learning, 5(3), 251–267. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9060-6
  • Kuhn, D., & Dean, Jr., D. (2004). Metacognition: A bridge between cognitive psychology and educational practice. Theory into Practice, 43(4), 268–274. doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4304_4
  • Linderholm, T., Everson, M. G., van den Broek, P., Mischinski, M., Crittenden, A., & Samuels, J. (2000). Effects of causal text revisions on more and less skilled readers’ comprehension of easy and difficult texts. Cognition and Instruction, 18(4), 525–556. doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI1804_4
  • Magno, C. (2010). The role of metacognitive skills in developing critical thinking. Metacognition Learning, 5(3), 137–156. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9054-4
  • Mall-Amiri, B., & Ahmadi, Z. (2014). The relationship between EFL learners’ critical thinking, and metacognitive strategies. International Journal of Language Learning and Applied Linguistics World (IJLLALW), 5(1), 374–387.
  • Mante, M. J. S. (2009). Dimensions of motivation to read and metacognitive reading strategies as predictors of reading comprehension among Filipino bilinguals. Unpublished Dissertation. De LaSalle University.
  • McCrudden, M. T., Schraw, G., Lehman, S., & Poliquin, A. (2007). The effect of causal diagrams on text learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(3), 367–388. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.11.002
  • McPeck, J. E. (1990). Critical thinking and subject specificity: A reply to Ennis. Educational Researcher, 19(4), 10–12. doi:10.3102/0013189X019004010
  • Mehrdad, A., Ahghar, M. R., & Ahghar, M. (2012). The effect of teaching cognitive and metacognitive strategies on EFL students’ reading comprehension across proficiency levels. Social and Behavioral Sciences, 46(7), 3757–3763.
  • Mehrpour, S., Sadighi, F., & Bagheri, Z. (2012). Teaching reading comprehension strategies to Iranian EFL pre-university students. The Journal of Teaching Language Skills (JTLS), 4(1), 107–139.
  • Mendenhall, A., & Johnson, T. E. (2010). Fostering the development of critical thinking skills, and reading comprehension of undergraduates using a web 2.0 tool coupled with a learning system. Journal of Interactive Learning Environments, 18(3), 263–276. doi:10.1080/10494820.2010.500537
  • Mokhtari, K., & Reichard, C. A. (2002). Assessing students’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(13), 249–259. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.249
  • Nemat-Tabrizi, A. R., & Erfani, L. (2014). The relationship between critical thinking and cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies among Iranian EFL learners. International Journal of Language Learning and Applied Linguistics World, 7(1), 265–277.
  • Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should know. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.
  • Pammu, A., Amir, Z., & Maasum, T. (2014). Metacognitive reading strategies of less proficient tertiary learners: A case study of EFL learners at a public university in Makassar, Indonesia. The Journal of Teaching Language Skills (JTLS), 8(2), 115–132.
  • Paul, R. W., Elder, L., & Batell, T. (1997). California teacher preparation for instruction in critical thinking: Research findings and policy recommendations. Sacramento, CA: California Commission on Teacher Credentialing, 1997.
  • Phakiti, A. (2003a). A closer look at the relationship of cognitive and metacognitive strategy use to EFL reading comprehension test performance. Language Testing, 20(5), 26–56. doi:10.1191/0265532203lt243oa
  • Phakiti, A. (2003b). A closer look at the relationship of cognitive and metacognitive strategy use to EFL reading achievement test performance. Language Testing, 20(1), 26–56. doi:10.1191/0265532203lt243oa
  • Pressley, M. (2002). Metacognition and self-regulated comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup, & S. J. Samuel (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 291-309). Newark: International Reading Association. doi:10.1598/0872071774.13
  • Riyadi, I., Hersulastuti, T., & Nugrahaningsih, K. (2017). Metacognitive learning strategy: In search of theoretical model for reading comprehension. SEEDS, 1(1), 125–142.
  • Saks, K., & Leigen, A. (2018). Cognitive and metacognitive strategies as predictors of language learning outcomes. Psihologija, 51(1), 1–17. doi:10.2298/PSI180121025S
  • Seifoori, Z. (2018). Postgraduate English students’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies and their reading comprehension: A comparative study. The Journal of Applied Linguistics and Discourse Analysis, 3(1), 117–134.
  • Shih, H., & Huang, S. (2018). EFL learners’ metacognitive strategy use in reading tests. English Teaching & Learning, 42(2), 117–130. doi:10.1007/s42321-018-0007-3
  • Silva, E. (2008). Measuring Skills for the 21st Century [Report]. Washington, DC: Education Sector. Retrieved from http://www.educationsector.org/usr_doc/MeasuringSkills.pdf
  • Tavakoli, H. (2014). The effectiveness of metacognitive strategy awareness in reading comprehension: The case of Iranian University EFL Students. The Reading Matrix, 14(2), 314–336.
  • Van Gelder, T. (2005). Teaching critical thinking: Some lessons from cognitive science. College Teaching, 53(1), 41–48. doi:10.3200/CTCH.53.1.41-48
  • Vandergrift, L. (1999). Facilitating second language listening comprehension: Acquiring successful strategies. ELT Journal, 53(3), 168–176. doi:10.1093/elt/53.3.168
  • Vandergrift, L., & Tafaghodtari, M. (2010). Teaching L2 learners how to listen does make a difference: An empirical study. Language Learning, 60(2), 470–497. doi:10.1111/(ISSN)1467-9922
  • VanPatten, B. (1996). Explanation vs. Structured input in processing instruction’. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 225–243. doi:10.1017/S0272263100011979
  • Wenden, A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge in SLA: The neglected variable. In M. P. Breen (Ed.), Learner contributions to language learning: New directions in research (pp. 44–46). Harlow: Pearson.
  • Willingham, D. T. (2007). Critical thinking: Why is it so hard to teach? American Educator, 8(3), 8–19.
  • Zare, A. (2007). The relationship between cognitive and meta-cognitive strategy use and EFL reading achievement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 2(5), 121–130.