125
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Word Stress in German Single-Word Reading

&

References

  • Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2006). The processing of lexical stress during visual word recognition: Typicality effects and orthographic correlates. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(5), 920–948.
  • Augst, G. (1985). Kinderwort: Der aktive Kinderwortschatz kurz vor der Einschulung [Child-word. Pre-school children's active vocabulary, arranged according to domains with an alphabetical index]. Frankfurt am Main: Lang.
  • Augst, G. (1989). Schriftwortschatz. Untersuchungen und Wortlisten Zum Orthographischen Lexikon bei Schülern und Erwachsenen [Examinations and glossaries of the orthographic lexicon for school children and adults]. Frankfurt am Main: Lang.
  • Baayen, R.H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database. [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Balota, D.A. (1994). Visual word recognition. The journey from features to meaning. In M.A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 303–358). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Bedoin, N., & dos Santos, C. (2008). How do consonant feature values affect the processing of a CVCV structure? Evidence from a reading task. Written Language and Literacy, 11(2), 191–210.
  • Borgwaldt, S., Hellwig, F., & de Groot, A. (2004). Word-initial entropy in five languages. Written Language and Literacy, 7(2), 165–184.
  • Colombo, L. (1988). Influence of stress assignment in reading Italian words. Contributi dei Dipartimenti e degli Istituti Italiani di Psicologia, 1(2), 69–77.
  • Colombo, L. (1991). The role of lexical stress in word recognition and pronunciation. Psychological Research, 53(1), 71–79.
  • Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(6), 1197–1211.
  • Cutler, A., & Carter, D.M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech and Language, 2(3), 133–142.
  • Ehri, L.C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18(2), 116–125.
  • Féry, C. (2001). Phonologie des Deutschen: Eine optimalitätstheoretische Einführung I, II [German phonology: An optimality-theoretic introduction]. Potsdam, Germany: Universitätsbibliothek, Publikationsstelle.
  • Fodor, J.D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27(2), 285–319.
  • Forster, K.I., & Forster, J.C. (2003). DMDX. A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 35(1), 116–124.
  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 71–99.
  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(1), 104–115.
  • Gibson, E.J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Meredith Corporation.
  • Giegerich, H.J. (1985). Metrical phonology and phonological structure: German and English. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Hayes, B. (1995): Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Lenhard, W., & Schneider, W. (2006). ELFE 1–6. Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst- bis Sechstklässler [ELFE 1-6: A reading comprehension test for school children from grade 1 to 6]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
  • Levelt, W.J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.
  • Janßen, U. (2004). Untersuchungen zum Wortakzent im Deutschen und Niederländischen [Investigations on word stress in German and Dutch] (Doctoral dissertation). University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany.
  • Kelly, M.H., Morris, J., & Verrekia, L. (1998). Orthographic cues to lexical stress: Effects on naming and lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 822–832.
  • Mengel, A. (2000). Deutscher Wortakzent: Symbole, Signale [German word stress: Symbols, signals]. München, Germany: Phorm.
  • Monsell, S., Doyle, M.C., & Haggard, P.N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: Where are they? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(1), 43–71.
  • Neef, M. (1998). A declarative path through the jungle of German noun inflection. (Working paper Sonderforschungsbereich 282, Theorie des Lexikons No. 102). University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany.
  • Noack, C. (2009). Can secondary pupils train decoding skills? An empirical study on phonological reading errors. Written Language and Literacy, 12(1), 97–115.
  • Nuerk, H., Rey, A., Graf, R., & Jacobs, A.M. (2000). Phonographic sublexical units in visual word recognition. Current Psychology Letters, 2(2), 3–29.
  • Penke, M., & Schrader, K. (2008). The role of phonology in written word recognition: Reading acquisition vs. skilled reading. Written Language & Literacy, 11(2), 167–190.
  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J.C., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: Large scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP++) model. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 106–151.
  • Rapp, B., Folk, J.R., & Tainturier, M. (2001). Word reading. In B. Rapp, (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 233–262). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42(3), 342–364.
  • Röber, C. (2008). Die Nutzung der phonologischen Informationen der Orthographie für das Lesen deutscher Wörter. Zur Problematik des Leseunterrichts in der Schule. [Using phonological information provided by orthography to read German words. On the difficulty of reading instructions in school] (Unpublished manuscript). Pädagogische Hochschule Freiburg.
  • Schiller, N.O., Jansma, B.M., Peters, J., & Levelt, W.J. M. (2006). Monitoring metrical stress in polysyllabic words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(1–3), 112–140.
  • Wiese, R. (1996). The phonology of German. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.
  • Ziegler, J.C., & Jacobs, A.M. (1995). Phonological information provides early sources of constraint in the processing of letter strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(5), 567–593.
  • Ziegler, J.C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 12(3), 413–430.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.