713
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Young children’s impressionable use of teleology: the influence of question wording and questioned topic on teleological explanations for natural phenomena

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 808-826 | Received 01 Sep 2017, Accepted 08 Mar 2018, Published online: 26 Mar 2018

References

  • Amin, T. G., Smith, C., & Wiser, M. (2014). Student conceptions and conceptual change: Three overlapping phases of research. In N. G. Lederman & S. K. Abell (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education (2nd ed., pp. 57–81). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Amundson, R., & Lauder, G. V. (1994). Function without purpose. Biology and Philosophy, 9, 443–469. doi: 10.1007/BF00850375
  • Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), Handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Dawes, L. (2004). Research report: Talk and learning in classroom science. International Journal of Science Education, 26, 677–695. doi: 10.1080/0950069032000097424
  • DfE. (2013). The national curriculum in England: Key stages 1 and 2 framework document. HMSO. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/national-curriculum
  • diSessa, A. A. (2014). A history of conceptual change research: Threads and fault lines. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (2nd ed., pp. 88–108). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • diSessa, A. A., Gillespie, N. M., & Esterly, J. B. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28, 843–900. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2806_1
  • DiYanni, Cara, & Kelemen, Deborah. (2005). Time to get a new mountain? The role of function in children's conceptions of natural kinds. Cognition, 97, 327–335. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.002
  • Halls, J., Ainsworth, S., & Oliver, M. (2016, June 20–24). The influence of question wording on children’s tendencies to provide teleological explanations for natural phenomena. In C. K. Looi, J. Polman, U. Cress, & P. Reimann (Eds.), Transforming learning, empowering learners: The international conference of the learning sciences (ICLS) 2016, Volume 2 (pp. 775–778). Singapore: International Society of the Learning Sciences. Retrieved from https://www.isls.org/icls/2016/docs/ICLS2016_Volume_2.pdf
  • Hanke, D. (2004). Teleology: The explanation that bedevils biology. In J. Cornwell (Ed.), Explanations: Styles of explanation in science (pp. 143–153). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Ioannides, C, & Vosniadou, S. (2002). The changing meanings of force. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2, 5–61.
  • Kampourakis, K. (2014). Understanding evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kampourakis, K., Palaiokrassa, E., Papadopoulou, M., Pavlidi, V., & Argyropoulou, M. (2012). Children’s intuitive teleology: Shifting the focus of evolution education research. Evolution: Education & Outreach, 5, 279–291.
  • Keil, F. C. (1992). The origins of an autonomous biology. In M. R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (pp. 103–138). Hilldale, NJ: Earlbaum.
  • Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 235–262). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Kelemen, D. (1999a). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70(3), 241–272. Retrieved from <Go to ISI>://WOS:000080712600002 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4
  • Kelemen, D. (1999b). Why are rocks pointy? Children’s preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1440–1452. Retrieved from <Go to ISI>://WOS:000083553000008 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1440
  • Kelemen, D. (2003). British and American children’s preferences for teleo-functional explanations of the natural world. Cognition, 88(2), 201–221. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00024-6
  • Kelemen, D. (2012). Teleological minds: How natural intuitions about agency and purpose influence leaning about evolution. In K. Rosengren, S. K. Brem, E. M. Evans, & G. M. Sinatra (Eds.), Evolution challenges: Integrating research and practice in teaching and learning about evolution (pp. 66–93). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Kelemen, D., & DiYanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children’s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6, 3–31. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0601_2
  • Lindeman, M., Svedholm-Hakkinen, A. M., & Lipsanen, J. (2015). Ontological confusions but not mentalizing abilities predict religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in supernatural purpose. Cognition, 134, 63–79. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.008
  • McGarrigle, J., & Donaldson, M. (1974). Some clues to the nature of semantic development. Journal of Child Language, 1, 185–194.
  • ojalehto, B., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2013). Teleological reasoning about nature: Intentional design or relational perspectives? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.006
  • Olson, D. R. (2003). Psychological theory and educational reform: How school remakes mind and society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783. doi: 10.1037/h0043424
  • Peacock, Graham, Sharp, John, Johnsey, Rob, & Wright, Debbie. (2011). Primary science: knowledge and understanding (5th). Exeter: Learning Matters.
  • Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of the world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  • Piaget, Jean. (1929). The child's conception of the world. London: Kegan Paul.
  • Polling, D. A., & Evans, E. M. (2002). Why do birds of a feather flock together? Developmental change in the use of multiple explanations: Intention, teleology and essentialism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 89–112. doi: 10.1348/026151002166343
  • Ruse, M. (1989). Teleology in biology: Is it a cause for concern? TREE, 4(2), 51–54.
  • Talanquer, V. (2007). Explanations and teleology in chemistry education. International Journal of Science Education, 29, 853–870. doi: 10.1080/09500690601087632
  • Tao, Y., Oliver, M., & Venville, G. J. (2011). Chinese and Australian year 3 children’s conceptual understanding of science: A multiple comparative case study. International Journal of Science Education, 34, 879–901. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2011.578679
  • Woodfield, A. (1976). Teleology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Zohar, A., & Ginossar, S. (1998). Lifting the taboo regarding teleological and anthropomorphism in biology education – Heretical suggestions. Science Education, 82, 679–697. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(199811)82:6<679::AID-SCE3>3.0.CO;2-E

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.