90
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Preschool Children’s Science Learning: Instructional Approaches and Individual Differences

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021017
  • Baker, H., Haubmann, A., Kloos, H., & Fisher, A. (2011). Preschoolers’ learning about buoyancy: Does it help to give away the answer? In Proceedings of the First Joint International Conference on Learning and Development and Epigenetic Robotics, IEEE, Frankfurt, Germany.
  • Bascandziev, I., Powell, L. J., Harris, P. L., & Carey, S. (2016). A role for executive functions in explanatory understanding of the physical world. Cognitive Development, 39, 71–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.04.001
  • Bascandziev, I., Tardiff, N., Zaitchik, D., & Carey, S. (2018). The role of domain-general cognitive resources in children’s construction of a vitalist theory of biology. Cognitive Psychology, 104, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.03.002
  • Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001
  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233
  • Bruner, J. S. (1961). The act of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31(1), 21–32.
  • Butts, D. P., Hoffman, H., & Anderson, M. (1993). Is hands-on experience enough? A study of young children’s views of sinking and floating objects. Journal of Elementary Science Education, 5(1), 50–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03170644
  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. MIT Press.
  • Carey, S. (2000). Science education as conceptual change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 13–19. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99)00046-5
  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.
  • Carlson, S. M. (2021). Minnesota executive function scale. (Technical Report). Reflection Sciences, Inc.
  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298
  • Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2014). Minnesota executive function scale. Reflection Sciences, LLC.
  • Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, Vol. 1: Body and mind (pp. 706–743). Oxford University Press.
  • Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., & Katch, L. E. (2004). Beyond the reading wars: Exploring the effect of child-instruction interactions on growth in early reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(4), 305–336. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0804_1
  • Curran, F. C., & Kellogg, A. T. (2016). Understanding science achievement gaps by race/ethnicity and gender in kindergarten and first grade. Educational Researcher, 45(5), 273–282. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16656611
  • Curran, F. C., & Kitchin, J. (2019). Early elementary science instruction: Does more time on science or science topics/skills predict science achievement in the early grades? American Educational Research Association Open, 5(3), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858419861081
  • Duncan, G. J., Yeung, W. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Smith, J. R. (1998). How much does childhood poverty affect the life chances of children? American Sociological Review, 63(3), 406–423. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657556
  • Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (2005). First grade and educational attainment by age 22: A new story. American Journal of Sociology, 110(5), 1458–1502. https://doi.org/10.1086/428444
  • Fisher, K. R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2013). Taking shape: Supporting preschoolers’ acquisition of geometric knowledge through guided play. Child Development, 84(6), 1872–1878. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12091
  • Grenell, A., & Carlson, S. M. (2021). Individual differences in young children’s executive function and learning: Role of type of knowledge and conflict with prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 206, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105079
  • Hardy, I., Jonen, A., Moller, K., & Stern, E. (2006). Effects of instructional support within constructivist learning environments for elementary school students’ understanding of “floating and sinking”. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 307–326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.307
  • Hardy, I., Saalbach, H., Leuchter, M., & Schalk, L. (2020). Preschoolers’ induction of the concept of material kind to make predictions: The effects of comparison and linguistic labels. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.531503
  • Hart, C. H., Burts, D. C., Durland, M. A., Charlesworth, R., DeWolf, M., & Fleege, P. O. (1998). Stress behaviors and activity type participation of preschoolers in more and less developmentally appropriate classrooms: SES and Sex differences. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 12(2), 176–196. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549809594883
  • Homer, B. D., & Plass, J. L. (2014). Level of interactivity and executive functions as predictors of learning in computer-based chemistry simulations. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 365–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.041
  • Hong, S. H., & Diamond, K. E. (2012). Two approaches to teaching young children science concepts, vocabulary, and scientific problem-solving skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 295–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.09.006
  • Honomichl, R. D., & Chen, Z. (2012). The role of guidance in children’s discovery learning. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3, 615–622. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1199 (6)
  • Hsin, C.-T., & Wu, H.-K. (2011). Using scaffolding strategies to promote young children’s scientific understandings of floating and sinking. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20(5), 656–666. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-011-9310-7
  • Kallery, M. (2015). Science in early years education: Introducing floating and sinking as a property of matter. International Journal of Early Years Education, 23(1), 31–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2014.999646
  • Kikas, E., Peets, K., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2014). Collective student characteristics alter the effects of teaching practices on academic outcomes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 273–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.04.004
  • Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
  • Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15(10), 661–667. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00737.x
  • Kloos, H., & Somerville, S. C. (2001). Providing impetus for conceptual change: The effect of organizing the input. Cognitive Development, 16(2), 737–759. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00053-3
  • Kloos, H., & van Orden, G. C. (2005). Can a preschooler’s mistaken belief benefit learning? Swiss Journal of Psychology, 64(3), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.64.3.195
  • Koerber, S., & Osterhaus, C. (2019). Individual differences in early scientific thinking: Assessment, cognitive influences, and their relevance to science learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(4), 510–533. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1620232
  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. Psychological Corporation.
  • Lee, H. S., & Anderson, J. R. (2013). Student learning: What has instruction got to do with it? Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 445–469. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143833
  • Leuchter, M., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2014). Designing science learning in the first years of schooling. An intervention study with sequenced learning material on the topic of ‘floating and sinking’. International Journal of Science Education, 36(10), 1751–1771. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2013.878482
  • Little, T. D., Jorgensen, T. D., Lang, K. M., & Moore, E. W. G. (2014). On the joys of missing data. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39(2), 151–162. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jst048
  • Mayer, R. E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychologist, 59(1), 14–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.14
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., & Maczuga, S. (2016). Science achievement gaps begin very early, persist, and are largely explained by modifiable factors. Educational Researcher, 45(1), 18–35. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16633182
  • Piaget, J. (1970). Science of education and the psychology of the child. ( D. Coltman, Trans.). Orion.
  • Piekny, J., Grube, D., & Maehler, C. (2013). The relation between preschool children’s false- belief understanding and domain-general experimentation skills. Metacognition and Learning, 8(2), 103–119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-013-9097-4
  • Piekny, J., & Maehler, C. (2013). Scientific reasoning in early and middle childhood: The development of domain-general evidence evaluation, experimentation, and hypothesis generation skills. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 153–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02082.x
  • R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. Child Development, 77(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00852.x
  • Roid, G. H. (2005). Stanford- Binet intelligence scales for early childhood manual (5th ed.). Riverside Publishing.
  • Siler,S., Klahr, D., Magaro, C., Willows, K., & Mowery, D. (2010). Predictors of transfer of experimental design skills in elementary and middle school children. In V. Aleven, J. Kay, & J. Mostow (Eds.), Intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 198–208). Springer.
  • Singer, J. A., Kohn, A. S., & Resnick, L. B. (1997). Knowing about proportions in different contexts. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An international perspective (pp. 115–132). Psychology Press.
  • Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75(3), 417–453. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075003417
  • Spybrook, J., Zhang, Q., Kelcey, B., & Dong, N. (2020). Learning from cluster randomized trials in education: An assessment of the capacity of studies to determine what works, for whom, and under what conditions. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 42(3), 354–374. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373720929018
  • Stipek, D. J., Feiler, R., Byler, P., Ryan, R., Milburn, S., & Salmon, J. M. (1998). Good beginnings: What difference does the program make in preparing young children for school? Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19(1), 41–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99)80027-6
  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1202_4
  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205
  • Tuovinen, J. E., & Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load associated with discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 334–341. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.334
  • Vaisarova, J., & Reynolds, A. J. (2022). Is more child-initiated always better? Exploring relations between child-initiated instruction and preschoolers’ school readiness. Educational Assessment, Evaluation and Accountability, 34(2), 195–226. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11092-021-09376-6
  • van Buuren, S. (2018). Flexible imputation of missing data (2nd ed.). Taylor & Francis.
  • van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 1–67. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03
  • van der Graaf, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Scientific reasoning in kindergarten: Cognitive factors in experimentation and evidence evaluation. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 190–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.06.006
  • van der Graaf, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Individual differences in the development of scientific thinking in kindergarten. Learning and Instruction, 56, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.03.005
  • van Ginkel, J. R., Linting, M., Rippe, R. C., & van der Voort, A. (2020). Rebutting existing misconceptions about multiple imputation as a method for handling missing data. Journal of Personality Assessment, 102(3), 297–308. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2018.1530680
  • Wagensveld, B., Segers, E., Kleemans, T., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Children predictors of learning to control variables via instruction or self-discovery. Instructional Science, 43(3), 365–379. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-014-9334-5
  • Weisberg, D. S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Kittredge, A. K., & Klahr, D. (2016). Guided play: Principles and practices. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 177–182. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416645512
  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43(1), 337–375. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.43.020192.002005
  • Yu, Y., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (2019). Pedagogical questions in parent-child conversations. Child Development, 90(1), 147–161. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12850
  • Yu, Y., Landrum, A. R., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (2018). Questioning supports effective transmission of knowledge and increased exploratory learning in pre-kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 21(6), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12696
  • Zeileis, A. (2004). Econometric computing with HC and HAC covariance matrix estimators. Journal of Statistical Software, 11(10), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v011.i10
  • Zeileis, A., Köll, S., & Graham, N. (2020). Various versatile variances: An object–oriented implementation of clustered covariances in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 95(1), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v095.i01

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.