803
Views
29
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Use of Contextual Information and Prediction by Struggling Adult Readers: Evidence From Reading Times and Event-Related Potentials

, , , &

References

  • Andrews, S., & Bond, R. (2009). Lexical expertise and reading skill: Bottom-up and top-down processing of lexical ambiguity. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 687–711. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9137-7
  • Baggio, G., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The balance between memory and unification in semantics: A dynamic account of the N400. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(9), 1338–1367. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.542671
  • Barr, D. J. (2013). Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 328. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00328
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory & Language, 68, 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain & Language, 40(4), 471–490. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(91)90144-P
  • Borovsky, A., Elman, J., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417–436. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005
  • Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, W. E. (2007). Speaking up for vocabulary: Reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(3), 226–243. doi:10.1177/00222194070400030401
  • Briggs, P., Austin, S., & Underwood, G. (1984). The effects of sentence context in good and poor readers: A test of Stanovich’s Interactive-Compensatory Model. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(1), 54–61. doi:10.2307/747651
  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 439–454. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.439
  • Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1983). Adult age differences in the effects of sentence context and stimulus degradation during visual word recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 239–251. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1983.tb01860.x
  • DeLong, K. A., Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2012). Thinking ahead or not? Natural aging and anticipation during reading. Brain & Language, 121(3), 226–239. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2012.02.006
  • Ditman, T., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). An investigation of concurrent ERP and self-paced reading methodologies. Psychophysiology, 44, 927–935. doi:10.1111/psyp.2007.44.issue-6
  • Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 353–387. doi:10.1017/S0272263112000915
  • Ehrlich, S., & Rayner, K. (1981). Contextual effects on word perception and eye movements during reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 641–655. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90220-6
  • Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44, 491–505. doi:10.1111/psyp.2007.44.issue-4
  • Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 469–495. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2660
  • Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2005). Aging in context: Age-related changes in context use during language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 42, 133–141. doi:10.1111/psyp.2005.42.issue-2
  • Federmeier, K. D., Kutas, M., & Schul, R. (2010). Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and Language, 115, 149–161. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.006
  • Federmeier, K. D., McLennan, D. B., De Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2002). The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 39(2), 133–146. doi:10.1111/psyp.2002.39.issue-2
  • Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., De Ochoa-Dewald, E., & Kutas, M. (2007). Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Research, 1146, 75–84. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.101
  • Gao, X., Stine-Morrow, E. A., Noh, S. R., & Eskew, Jr., R. T. (2011). Visual noise disrupts conceptual integration in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 83–88. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0014-4
  • Hall, R., Greenberg, D., Laures-Gore, J., & Pae, H. K. (2014). The relationship between expressive vocabulary knowledge and reading skills for adult struggling readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(S1), S87–S100. doi:10.1111/jrir.v37.S1
  • Hartshorne, J. K., & Germine, L. T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433–443. doi:10.1177/0956797614567339
  • Hersch, J., & Andrews, S. (2012). Lexical quality and reading skill: Bottom-up and top-down contributions to sentence processing. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(3), 240–262. doi:10.1080/10888438.2011.564244
  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160. doi:10.1007/BF00401799
  • Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition & Neuroscience, 31(1), 19–31. doi:10.1080/23273798.2015.1072223
  • Kendeou, P., Savage, R., & Van Den Broek, P. (2009). Revisiting the simple view of reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 353–370. doi:10.1348/978185408X369020
  • Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do you mean by prediction in language comprehension. Language, Cognition & Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. doi:10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. doi:10.1126/science.7350657
  • Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2009). A beautiful day in the neighborhood: An event-related potential study of lexical relationships and prediction in context. Journal of Memory & Language, 61(3), 326–338. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.06.004
  • Madden, D. (1988). Adult age differences in the effects of sentence context and stimulus degradation during visual word recognition. Psychology and Aging, 3(2), 167–172. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.3.2.167
  • Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J.-R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory & Language, 69, 574–588. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.001
  • McClelland, J. L. (1987). The case for interactionism in language processing. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 3–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Miller, L. M., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (1998). Aging and the effects of knowledge on on-line reading strategies. The Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53(4), P223–P233. doi:10.1093/geronb/53B.4.P223
  • Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F. (2012). Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eyemovements are modulated by reading ability - Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1), 3, 1–10.
  • Morris, R. K. (2006). Lexical processing and sentence context effects. In M. J. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 377–401). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178. doi:10.1037/h0027366
  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Individual differences in contextual factilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69(4), 996–1011. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06157.x
  • National Research Council. (2012). Improving adult literacy instruction: Options for practice and research. Committee on Learning Sciences: Foundations and Applications to Adolescent and Adult Literacy. In A. M. Lesgold and M. Welch-Ross (Eds.), Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
  • Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–452. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431
  • Payne, B. R., & Federmeier, D. K. (2017). Pace yourself: Intraindividual variability in context use revealed by self-paced event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01090
  • Payne, B. R., Gao, X., Noh, S. R., Anderson, C. J., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2012). The effects of print exposure on sentence processing and memory in older adults: Evidence for efficiency and reserve. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition, 19(1–2), 122–149. doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.628376
  • Payne, B. R., Lee, C. L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2015). Revisiting the incremental effects of context on word processing: Evidence from single-word event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 52(11), 1456–1469. doi:10.1111/psyp.12515
  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730
  • Perfetti, C., Goldman, S. R., & Hogaboam, T. W. (1979). Reading skills and the identifcation of words in discourse context. Memory & Cognition, 7(4), 272–282. doi:10.3758/BF03197600
  • Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 22–37. doi:10.1080/10888438.2013.827687
  • Perfetti, C., Yang, C. L., & Schmalhofer, F. (2008). Comprehension skill and word-to-text integration processes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 303–318. doi:10.1002/acp.1419
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12001495
  • Richter, T., Isberner, M.-B., Naumann, J., & Neeb, Y. (2013). Lexical quality and reading comprehension in primary school children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 415–434. doi:10.1080/10888438.2013.764879
  • Schwanenflugel, P., & Shoben, E. J. (1985). The influence of sentence constraint on the scope of facilitation for upcoming words. Journal of Memory & Language, 24, 232–252. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(85)90026-9
  • Schwantes, F. M., Boesl, S. L., & Ritz, E. G. (1980). Children’s use of context in word recognition: A psycholinguistic guessing game. Child Development, 51(3), 730–736. doi:10.2307/1129458
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an Interactive-Compensatory Model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16(1), 32–71. doi:10.2307/747348
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1981). The effect of sentence context on ongoing word recognition: Tests of a two-process theory. Journal of Experimenal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(3), 658–672.
  • Steen, A. A., Ng, S., Payne, B. R., Anderson, C. J., Federmeier, D. K., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (under review). The effects of context on processing words during sentence reading among adults varying in age and literacy skill. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Miller, L. M., & Hertzog, C. (2006). Aging and self-regulated language processing. Psychological Bulletin, 132(4), 582–606. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.582
  • Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Payne, B. R. (2016). Age differences in language segmentation. Experimental Aging Research, 42(1), 107–125. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2016.1108751
  • Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Soederberg Miller, L. M., Gagne, D. D., & Hertzog, C. (2008). Self-regulated reading in adulthood. Psychology & Aging, 23(1), 131–153. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.131
  • Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Soederberg Miller, L. M., & Nevin, J. A. (1999). The effects of context and feedback on age differences in spoken word recognition. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54(2), P125–P134. doi:10.1093/geronb/54B.2.P125
  • West, R., & Stanovich, K. E. (1978). Contextual facilitation in readers of three ages. Child Development, 49(3), 717–727. doi:10.2307/1128240
  • Wlotko, E. W., & Federmeier, K. D. (2012). Age-related changes in the impact of contextual strength on multiple aspects of sentence comprehension. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 770–785. doi:10.1111/psyp.2012.49.issue-6
  • Wlotko, E. W., & Federmeier, D. K. (2015). Time for prediction? The effect of presentation rate on predictive sentence comprehension during word-by-word reading. Cortex, 68, 20–32. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.014
  • Wlotko, E. W., Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2012). To predict or not to predict: Age-related differences in the use of sentential context. Psychology & Aging, 27(4), 975–988. doi:10.1037/a0029206

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.