1,109
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Does the bilingual advantage extend to trilingualism?

&
Pages 549-562 | Received 14 Sep 2018, Accepted 07 May 2019, Published online: 23 May 2019

References

  • Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003
  • Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigrations status. Neurology, 81(2), 1938–1944. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4
  • Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75(6), 959–963. doi: 10.1002/ana.24158
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34(4), 859–873.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1341–1354.
  • Bialystok, E., & DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 565–574. doi: 10.1037/a0012735
  • Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and Aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 696–705. doi: 10.1037/a0037254
  • Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007
  • Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2014). Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in stimulus-stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 610–629. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000564
  • Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition, and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14, 256–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x
  • The British Tinnitus Association. (2015). All about tinnitus [online]. Retrieved from http://www.tinnitus.org.uk/documents/34
  • Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H. (2010). Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 24(2), 118–125. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181ca1221
  • Coderre, E. L., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2014). Electrophysiological explorations of the bilingual advantage: Evidence from a Stroop task. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e103424. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103424
  • Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013
  • De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. doi: 10.1177/0956797614557866
  • Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1201–1206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x
  • Engel de Abreu, P. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory (Hove, England), 19(5), 529–537. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.590504
  • Engel de Abreu, P., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Sciences, 23(11), 1364–1371. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443836
  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Vinas-Guasch, N., … Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from pre-schoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011
  • Gevins, A. S., & Cutillo, B. S. (1993). Neuroelectric evidence for distributed processing in human working memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 128–143. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90119-G
  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
  • Hartshorne, J. K., & Germine, L. T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433–443. doi: 10.1177/0956797614567339
  • Heeren, A., Maurage, P., Perrot, H., De Volder, A., Renier, L., Araneda, R., … Philippot, P. (2014). Tinnitus specifically alters the top-down executive control sub-component of attention: Evidence from the attention network task. Behavioural Brain Research, 269, 147–154. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.043
  • Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 315–325. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990010
  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(4), 625–658. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7
  • Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Lauber, E. J., Awh, E., Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(4), 462–475. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.462
  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
  • Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 70–78. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70
  • Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V. (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.929580
  • Klein, R. M. (2015). On the belief that the cognitive exercise associated with the acquisition of a second language enhances extra-linguistic cognitive functions: Is “type-I incompetence” at work here? Cortex, 73, 340–341. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.020
  • Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). L2 vocabulary: The interface between learning and representation. The Mental Lexicon, 3(3), 349–374. doi: 10.1075/ml.3.3.06lin
  • Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. doi: 10.1037/a0030875
  • Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(01), 81–93. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458
  • Mor, B., Yitzhaki-Amsalem, S., & Prior, A. (2014). The joint effect of bilingualism and ADHD on executive functions. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(6), 1–15.
  • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
  • Morton, J. B. (2015). Still waiting for real answers. Cortex, 73, 352–353. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.010
  • Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
  • Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 597–616. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2010.488288
  • Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-Back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
  • Paap, K. R., Hunter, A. J., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 615–639. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.944914
  • Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 5, 962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962
  • Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 313–325.
  • Perquin, M., Vaillant, M., Schuller, A., Pastore, J., Dartigues, J., Lair, M., … Group, M. (2013). Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: New evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis. PLoS One, 8(4), e62030. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062030
  • Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review, 35, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003
  • Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–551. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
  • Ratiu, I., & Azuma, T. (2015). Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.976226
  • Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 507–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023
  • Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2011). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(1), 26–37. doi: 10.1177/1367006910371021
  • Schroeder, S. R., & Marian, V. (2012). A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 591–601. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.669367
  • Schroeder, S. R., & Marian, V. (2017). Cognitive consequences of trilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 754–773. doi: 10.1177/1367006916637288
  • Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123
  • Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39(1), 100–110. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3910100
  • Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 412–422. doi: 10.1017/S1366728910000611

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.