694
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLES

The Communication of Naïve Theories of the Social World in Parent–Child Conversation

&

REFERENCES

  • Abrams, D., & Rutland, A. (2008). The development of subjective group dynamics. In S. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 47–65). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Abrams, D., Rutland, A., & Cameron, L. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: Children's judgments of normative and deviant in-group and out-group individuals. Child Development, 74, 1840–1856.
  • Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Ferrell, J., & Pelletier, J. (2008). Children's judgments of disloyal and immoral peer behavior: Subjective group dynamics in minimal intergroup contexts. Child Development, 79, 444–461.
  • Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Ferrell, J., & Pelletier, J. (2009). Children's group nous: Understanding and applying peer exclusion within and between groups. Child Development, 80, 224–243.
  • Astuti, R., Solomon, G. E. A., & Carey, S. (2004). Constraints on conceptual development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69, vii–135.
  • Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 547–609.
  • Baillargeon, R. (2008). Innate ideas revisited: For a principle of persistence in infants’ physical reasoning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 2–13.
  • Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.
  • Bar-Haim, Y., Ziv, T., Lamy, D., & Hodes, R. M. (2006). Nature and nurture in own-race face processing. Psychological Science, 17, 159–163.
  • Beals, D. E. (1997). Sources of support for learning words in conversation: Evidence from mealtimes. Journal of Child Language, 24, 673–694.
  • Berndt, T. J., & Heller, K. A. (1986). Gender stereotypes and social inferences: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 889–898.
  • Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity: A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 351–365.
  • Bohannon, J. N., & Stanowicz, L. B. (1988). The issue of negative evidence: Adult responses to children's language errors. Developmental Psychology, 24, 684–689.
  • Brewer, M. (2007). The social psychology of intergroup relations: Social categorization, ingroup bias, and out-group prejudice. In A. Kruglanski & T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 695–715). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Callanan, M. A., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2004). Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents’ language and children's developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology, 40, 746–763.
  • Canfield, C. F., & Ganea, P. A. (2014). ‘You could call it magic': What parents and siblings tell preschoolers about unobservable entities. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 269–286.
  • Chalik, L., & Rhodes, M. (2014). Preschoolers use social allegiances to predict behavior. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 136–160.
  • Clark, E. V. (2010). Adult offer, word-class, and child uptake in early lexical acquisition. First Language, 30, 250–269.
  • Cohen, T., Montoya, R., & Insko, C. (2006). Group morality and inter-group relations: Cross-cultural and experimental evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 1559–1572.
  • Diesendruck, G. (2003). Categories for names or names for categories? The interplay between domain-specific conceptual structure and language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 759–787.
  • Diesendruck, G., & haLevi, H. (2006). The role of language, appearance, and culture in children's social category-based induction. Child Development, 77, 539–553.
  • Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 248–253.
  • Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of ‘minimal’ group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793–811.
  • Gelman, S., Collman, P., & Maccoby, E. (1986). Inferring properties from categories versus inferring categories from properties: The case of gender. Child Development, 57, 396–404.
  • Gelman, S., & Noles, N. (2011). Domains and naïve theories. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 490–502.
  • Gelman, S., Taylor, M., & Nguyen, S. (2004). Mother–child conversations about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69, 1–145.
  • Gil-White, F. J. (2001). Are ethnic groups biological ‘species’ to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology, 42, 515–554.
  • Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research and policy implications. Science, 337, 1623–1627.
  • Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108.
  • Greene, J. (2003). From neural ‘is’ to moral ‘ought': What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 847–852.
  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2008). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. Carruthers S. Laurence & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Vol. 3. Foundations and the future (pp. 367–391). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24, 589–594.
  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.
  • Helwig, C. C. (2006). Rights, civil liberties, and democracy across cultures. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 185–210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 12577–12580.
  • Kinzler, K., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children's social preferences. Social Cognition, 27, 623–634.
  • LaBounty, J., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S., Lagattuta, K., & Liu, D. (2008). Mothers’ and fathers’ use of internal state talk with their young children. Social Development, 17, 757–775.
  • Lagattuta, K. H., & Wellman, H. M. (2002). Differences in early parent–child conversations about negative versus positive emotions: Implications for the development of psychological understanding. Developmental Psychology, 38, 564–580.
  • Levine, M., Cassidy, C., Brazier, G., & Reicher, S. (2002). Self-categorization and bystander non-intervention: Two experimental studies. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1452–1463.
  • Levine, M., & Thompson, K. (2004). Identity, place, and bystander intervention: Social categories and helping after natural disasters. Journal of Social Psychology, 144, 229–245.
  • Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Friends or foes: Infants use shared evaluations to infer others’ social relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 966–971.
  • Luce, M. R., & Callanan, M. A. (2010). Parents’ object labeling: Possible links to conventionality of word meaning. First Language, 30, 270–286.
  • Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children's attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77, 847–860.
  • Powell, L. J., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Preverbal infants expect members of social groups to act alike. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 3965–3972.
  • Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31, 1109–1121.
  • Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, 57–75.
  • Rhodes, M. (2012). Naïve theories of social groups. Child Development, 83, 1900–1916.
  • Rhodes, M. (2013). How two intuitive theories shape the development of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 12–16.
  • Rhodes, M. (2014). Children's explanations as a window into their intuitive theories of the social world. Cognitive Science, 38, 1687–1697.
  • Rhodes, M., & Brickman, D. (2011). The influence of competition on children's social categories. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 194–221.
  • Rhodes, M., & Chalik, L. (2013). Social categories as markers of intrinsic interpersonal obligations. Psychological Science, 24, 999–1006.
  • Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). A developmental examination of the conceptual structure of animal, artifact, and human social categories across two cultural contexts. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 244–274.
  • Rhodes, M., Leslie, S. J., & Tworek, C. (2012). Cultural transmission of social essentialism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 13526–13531.
  • Rigney, J. C., & Callanan, M. A. (2011). Patterns in parent–child conversations about animals at a marine science center. Cognitive Development, 26, 155–171.
  • Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1992). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds?. In G. R. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), Language, interaction and social cognition (pp. 11–36). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Callanan, M. A. (1998). Metarepresentation in action: 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds’ developing theories of mind in parent–child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 34, 491–502.
  • Salmon, K., Mewton, L., Pipe, M., & McDonald, S. (2011). Asking parents to prepare children for an event: Altering parental instructions influences children's recall. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 80–102.
  • Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS One, 6, 1–7. PMC3188955
  • Shutts, K., Roben, C., & Spelke, E. (2013). Children's use of social categories in thinking about people and social relationships. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 35–62.
  • Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1990). Culture and moral development. In J. W. Stigler R. A. Shweder & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 130–204). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23, 196–204.
  • Smetana, J. G. (1985). Preschool children's conceptions of transgressions: Effects of varying moral and conventional domain-related attributes. Developmental Psychology, 21, 18–29.
  • Smetana, J. G., & Killen, M. (2008). Moral cognitions, emotions, and neuroscience: An integrative developmental view. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2, 324–339.
  • Smith, C., Carey, S., & Wiser, M. (1985). On differentiation: A case study of the development of the concepts of size, weight, and density. Cognition, 21, 177–237.
  • Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. F. H., Yun, J., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18, 40–66.
  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89–96.
  • Spielman, D. A. (2000). Young children, minimal groups and dichotomous categorization. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1433–1441.
  • Taylor, M. G. (1996). The development of children's beliefs about social and biological aspects of gender differences. Child Development, 67, 1555–1571.
  • Taylor, M., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. (2009). Boys will be boys; cows will be cows: Children's essentialist reasoning about gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 80, 461–481.
  • Turnbull, W., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Racine, T. P. (2008). Relations between mother–child talk and 3- to 5-year-old children's understanding of belief: Beyond mental state terms to talk about the mind. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 367–385.
  • Wainryb, C. (2006). Moral development in culture: Diversity, tolerance, and justice. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 211–240). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Waxman, S. (2010). Names will never hurt me? Naming and the development of racial and gender categories in preschool-aged children. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 593–610.
  • Waxman, S. R., & Grace, A. D. (2012). Developing gender- and race-based categories in infants: Evidence from 7- and 11-month-olds. In G. Hayes & M. Bryant (Eds.), Psychology of culture (pp. 159–176). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 523–573). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.
  • Yau, J., & Smetana, J. G. (2003). Conceptions of moral, socio-conventional, and personal events among Chinese preschoolers in Hong Kong. Child Development, 74, 647–658.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.