199
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Visual Cues Generated During Action Facilitate 14-Month-Old Infants’ Mental Rotation

&

REFERENCES

  • Acredolo, L. P., Adams, A., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1984). The role of self-produced movement and visual tracking in infant spatial orientation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 312–327. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(84)90128-0
  • Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(3), 1–162. doi:10.2307/116199
  • Almeida, J., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2010). The role of the dorsal visual processing stream in tool identification. Psychological Science, 21, 772–778. doi:10.1177/0956797610371343
  • Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R., & Flom, R. (2004). Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 99–102. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00283.x
  • Bai, D. L., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1992). Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215–226. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03608.x
  • Bushnell, E. W., & Bourdreau, J. P. (1993). Motor development and the mind: The potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development. Child Development, 64, 1005–1021. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04184.x
  • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1
  • Campos, J. J., Bertenthal, B. I., & Kermoian, R. (1992). Early experience and emotional development: The emergence of wariness of heights. Psychological Science, 3, 61–64. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00259.x
  • Eppler, M. A. (1995). Development of manipulatory skills and the deployment of attention. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 391–405. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(95)90029-2
  • Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Walser, S., & Mast, F. W. (2009). Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 18–40. doi:10.1080/15248370902966719
  • Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Wilson, M., & Wilkening, F. (2009). Effects of action on children’s and adults’ mental imagery. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 34–51. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.003
  • Frick, A., & Wang, S. (2014). Mental spatial transformations in 14- and 16-month-old infants: Effects of action and observational experience. Child Development, 85, 278–293. doi:10.1111/cdev.12116
  • Funk, M., Brugger, P., & Wilkening, F. (2005). Motor processes in children’s imagery: The case of mental rotation of hands. Developmental Science, 8, 402–408. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00428.x
  • Gerson, S. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Learning from their own actions: The unique effect of producing actions on infants’ understanding. Child Development, 85, 264–277. doi:10.1111/cdev.12115
  • Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.000245
  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 20–25. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
  • Harman, K. L., Humphrey, G. K., & Goodale, M. A. (1999). Active manual control of object views facilitates visual recognition. Current Biology, 9, 1315–1318. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(00)80053-6
  • Haxby, J. V., Grady, C. L., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R. E., … Rapoport, S. I. (1991). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 88, 1621–1625. doi:10.1073/pnas.88.5.1621
  • Hespos, S. J., & Rochat, P. (1997). Dynamic mental representation in infancy. Cognition, 64, 153–188. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00029-2
  • James, K., Jones, S., Swain, S., Pereira, A., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Some views are better than others: Evidence for a visual bias in object views self-generated by toddlers. Developmental Science, 17, 338–353. doi:10.1111/desc.12124
  • James, K. H., & Atwood, T. (2009). Active motor experience changes neural activation patterns to letter-like symbols. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 91–110.
  • James, K. H., & Bose, P. (2011). Self-generated actions during learning objects and sounds create sensori-motor systems in the developing brain. Cognition, Brain & Behavior, 15, 485–503.
  • James, K. H., & Swain, S. N. (2011). Only self-generated actions create sensori-motor systems in the developing brain. Developmental Science, 14, 673–687. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01011.x
  • James, K. H., Swain, S. N., Jones, S. S., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Young children’s self-generated object views and object recognition. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 391–401. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.749481
  • Johnson, M. H., Mareschal, D., & Csibra, G. (2001). The functional development and integration of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways: A neurocomputational approach. In C.A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), The handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 339–351). MIT Press.
  • Krüger, M., & Krist, H. (2009). Imagery and motor processes—when are they connected? The mental transformation of body parts in development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 239–261. doi:10.1080/152483707903389341
  • Libertus, K., & Needham, A. (2010). Teach to reach: The effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Research, 50, 2750–2757. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.001
  • Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception–action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71, 137–144. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00127
  • Möhring, W., & Frick, A. (2013). Touching up mental rotation: Effects of manual experience on 6-month-old infants’ mental object rotation. Child Development, 84, 1554–1565. doi:10.1111/cdev.12065
  • Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19, 1063–1066. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02200.x
  • Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., & Rivière, J. (2014). Self-locomotion and spatial language and spatial cognition: Insights from typical and atypical development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 521. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00521
  • Perone, S., Madole, K. L., & Oakes, L. M. (2011). Learning how actions function: The role of outcomes in infants’ representation of events. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 351–362. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.02.006
  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). London, England: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1948)
  • Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19, 1067–1070. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02201.x
  • Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants: The impact of posture and its development on early eye–hand coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 24, 210–220. doi:10.1080/00222895.1992.9941616
  • Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1996). Tracking and anticipation of invisible spatial transformations by 4- to 8-month-old infants. Cognitive Development, 11, 3–17. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90025-8
  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action, and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946. doi:10.1037/a0027839
  • Ruff, H. A. (1989). The infant’s use of visual and haptic information in the perception and recognition of objects. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 43, 302–319. doi:10.1037/h0084222
  • Schwarzer, G., Freitag, C., & Schum, N. (2013). How crawling and manual object exploration are related to the mental rotation abilities of 9-month-old infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00097
  • Smith, L., & Gasser, M. (2005). The development of embodied cognition: Six lessons from babies. Artificial Life, 11, 13–29. doi:10.1162/1064546053278973
  • Smith, L. B. (2009). From fragments to geometric shape: Changes in visual object recognition between 18 and 24 months. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 290–294. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01654.x
  • Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: Motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Developmental Psychology, 46, 129–138. doi:10.1037/a0014618
  • Soska, K. C., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Development of three-dimensional completion of complex objects. Infancy, 18, 325–344. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00127.x
  • Street, S. Y., James, K. H., Jones, S. S., & Smith, L. B. (2011). Vision for action in toddlers: The posting task. Child Development, 82, 2083–2094. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01655.x
  • Teramoto, W., & Riecke, B. E. (2010). Dynamic visual information facilitates object recognition from novel viewpoints. Journal of Vision, 10, 11. doi:10.1167/10.13.11
  • Valyear, K. F., Culham, J. C., Sharif, N., Westwood, D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). A double dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in the ventral and dorsal visual streams: A human fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 218–228. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.004
  • Walle, E. A., & Campos, J. J. (2014). Infant language development is related to the acquisition of walking. Developmental Psychology, 50, 336–348. doi:10.1037/a0033238
  • Wilcox, T., Woods, R., Chapa, C., & McCurry, S. (2007). Multisensory exploration and object individuation in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 479–495. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.479

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.