796
Views
20
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Mapping Among Number Words, Numerals, and Nonsymbolic Quantities in Preschoolers

, &

References

  • Anderson, U. S., & Cordes, S. (2013). 1<2 and 2<3: Nonlinguistic appreciations of numerical order. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00005
  • Bar-David, E., Compton, E., Drennan, L., Finder, B., Grogan, K., & Leonard, J. (2009). Nonverbal number knowledge in preschool-age children. Mind matters. The Wesleyan Journal of Psychology, 4, 51–64.
  • Baroody, A. J., & Price, J. (1983). The development of the number–word sequence in the counting of three-year-olds. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 14, 361–368. doi:10.2307/748681
  • Barth, H., La Mont, K., Lipton, J., & Spelke, E. (2005). Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 14116–14121. doi:10.1073/pnas.0505512102
  • Barth, H., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. Developmental Science, 14, 125–135. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x
  • Benoit, L., Lehalle, H., Molina, M., Tijus, C., & Jouen, F. (2013). Young children’s mapping between arrays, number words, and digits. Cognition, 129, 95–101. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.005
  • Bialystok, E. (2000). Symbolic representation across domains in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 173–189. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2548
  • Bialystok, E., & Codd, J. (1996). Developing representations of quantity. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 28, 281–291. doi:10.1037/0008-400x.28.4.281
  • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Notation to symbol: Development in children’s understanding of print. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 223–243. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00138-3
  • Brannon, E. M. (2002). The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy. Cognition, 83, 223–240. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00005-7
  • Cantlon, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychological Science, 17, 401–406. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01719.x
  • Condry, K. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). The development of language and abstract concepts: The case of natural number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 22–38. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.22
  • Cordes, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2008). Quantitative competencies in infancy. Developmental Science, 11, 803–808. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00770.x
  • Cramer, D., & Howitt, D. L. (2004). The Sage dictionary of statistics: A practical resource for students in the social sciences. London, UK: Sage.
  • Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N
  • Frye, D., Braisby, N., Lowe, J., Maroudas, C., & Jon, N. (1989). Young children’s understanding of counting and cardinality. Child Development, 60, 1158–1171. doi:10.2307/1130790
  • Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children’s counting and concepts of number. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • Fuson, K. C., Secada, W. G., & Hall, J. W. (1983). Matching, counting, and conservation of numerical equivalence. Child Development, 54, 91–97. doi:10.2307/1129865
  • Gallistel, C. R. (1990). Representations in animal cognition: An introduction. Cognition, 37, 1–22. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90016-D
  • Gelman, R. (1993). A rational-constructivist account of early learning about numbers and objects. Advances in research theory. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 61–96). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Gelman, R., & Tucker, M. F. (1975). Further investigations of the young child’s conception of number. Child Development, 46, 167–175. doi:10.2307/1128845
  • Goldstein, A., Cole, T., & Cordes, S. (2016). How parents read counting books and non-numerical books to their preverbal infants: An observational study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1100, 1–10.
  • Gunderson, E. A., & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14, 1021–1032. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x
  • Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42, 335–346. doi:10.1016/0167-2789(90)90087-6
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Hughes, M. (1983). Teaching arithmetic to pre-school children. Educational Review, 35, 163–173. doi:10.1080/0013191830350207
  • Hughes, M. (1986). Children and number. New York, NY: Basil Blackwell.
  • Huntley-Fenner, G. (2001). Children’s understanding of number is similar to adults’ and rats’: Numerical estimation by 5-7-year-olds. Cognition, 78, B27–B40. doi:10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00122-0
  • Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105, 395–438. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005
  • Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E. M., & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 130–169. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.07.002
  • Leibovich, T., & Ansari, D. (2016). The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence, and outstanding questions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 70, 12–23. doi:10.1037/cep0000070
  • Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Symbolic estrangement: Evidence against a strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 635–641. doi:10.1037/a0027248
  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1–22. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.111.1.1
  • McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1324–1333. doi:10.3758/BF03192949
  • McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2004). Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 15, 776–781. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x
  • McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2007). Ratio abstraction by 6-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 740–745. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01969.x
  • McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 400–408. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.013
  • Michie, S. (1984). Why preschoolers are reluctant to count spontaneously. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 347–358. doi:10.1111/bjdp.1984.2.issue-4
  • Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J. J., & Zhang, H. C. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence—The role of the number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 56–60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00305.x
  • Mix, K. S. (2008). Surface similarity and label knowledge impact early numerical comparisons. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 13–32. doi:10.1348/026151007X189109
  • Mix, K. S. (2009). How Spencer made number: First uses of the number words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 427–444. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.11.003
  • Mix, K. S., Sandhofer, C. M., Moore, J. A., & Russell, C. (2012). Acquisition of the cardinal word principle: The role of input. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 274–283. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.10.003
  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. doi:10.1038/2151519a0
  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1973). Determinants of reaction time for digit inequality judgments. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 167–168. doi:10.3758/BF03334328
  • Mundy, E., & Gilmore, C. K. (2009). Children’s mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 490–502. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003
  • Nunez, R. E., & Lakoff, G. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Odic, D., Le Corre, M., & Halberda, J. (2015). Children’s mappings between number words and the approximate number system. Cognition, 138, 102–121. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.008
  • Opfer, J. E., Thompson, C. A., & Furlong, E. E. (2010). Early development of spatial–numeric associations: Evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers. Developmental Science, 13, 761–771. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00934.x
  • Palmer, A., & Baroody, A. J. (2011). Blake’s development of the number words ‘one,’ ‘two,’ and ‘three.’ Cognition and Instruction, 29, 265–296. doi:10.1080/07370008.2011.583370
  • Pikulski, J. J., & Chard, D. J. (2005). Fluency: Bridge between decoding and reading comprehension. The Reading Teacher, 58, 510–519. doi:10.1598/RT.58.6.2
  • Posid, T., & Cordes, S. (2015). Verbal counting moderates perceptual biases found in children’s cardinality judgments. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(4), 621–637. doi:10.1080/15248372.2014.934372
  • Sarnecka, B. W., & Carey, S. (2008). How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition, 108, 662–674. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.007
  • Sarnecka, B. W., & Lee, M. D. (2009). Levels of number knowledge during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 325–337. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.007
  • Schaeffer, B., Eggleston, V. H., & Scott, J. L. (1974). Number development in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 357–379. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90017-6
  • Siegel, L. S. (1974). Heterogeneity and spatial factors as determinants of numeration ability. Child Development, 45, 532–534. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1974.tb00632.x
  • Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237–250. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02438
  • Sinclair, A., Siegrist, F., & Sinclair, H. (1983). Young children’s ideas about the written numeral system. In D. Rogers & J. Sloboda (Eds.) The Acquisition of Symbolic Skills (pp. 535–542). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Susperreguy, M.-I., & Davis-Kean, P. E. (2015). Socialization of maths in the home environment using voice recordings to study maths talk. Studies in Psychology, 36, 643–655.
  • Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–398. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_3
  • Wagner, J. B., & Johnson, S. C. (2011). An association between understanding cardinality and analog magnitude representations in preschoolers. Cognition, 119, 10–22. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.014
  • Wylie, J., Jordan, J.-A., & Mulhern, G. (2012). Strategic development in exact calculation: Group and individual differences in four achievement subtypes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 112–130. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.05.005
  • Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition, 36, 155–193. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3
  • Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220–251. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-P
  • Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 74, B1–B11. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00066-9

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.