152
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Negative Numbers are not yet Automatically Associated with Space in 6th Graders

, , , &

References

  • Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & McDonough Ryan, P. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from arabic numerals: Developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 286–308. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2518
  • Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron, 35, 625–641. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9
  • Cipora, K., Patro, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2015). Are spatial-numerical associations a cornerstone for arithmetic learning? The lack of genuine correlations suggests no. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9(4), 190–206. doi:10.1111/mbe.12093
  • Cipora, K., Schroeder, P. A., Soltanlou, M., & Nuerk, H.-C. (in press). More space, better math: Is space a powerful tool or a cornerstone for understanding arithmetic? Visualizing Mathematics: the Role of Spatial Reasoning in Mathematical Thought, 1–65. doi:10.31234/osf.io/3vg9p
  • Colling, L. J., Szűcs, D., Ansari, D., Bryce, D., Becková, A., Bell, R., … Zwaan, R. A. (in-principle acceptance, registered report). A multilab preregistered Replication of the attentional SNARC effect. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
  • Crollen, V., Dormal, G., Seron, X., Lepore, F., & Collignon, O. (2013). Embodied numbers: The role of vision in the development of number–space interactions. Cortex, 49(1), 276–283. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.11.006
  • Dackermann, T., Fischer, U., Nuerk, H.-C., Cress, U., & Moeller, K. (2017). Applying embodied cognition: From useful interventions and their theoretical underpinnings to practical applications. ZDM, 49(4), 545–557. doi:10.1007/s11858-017-0850-z
  • De Hevia, M. D., Girelli, L., Addabbo, M., & Cassia, V. M. (2014). Human infants’ preference for left-to-right oriented increasing numerical sequences. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e96412. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096412
  • de Hevia, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children. Cognition, 110(2), 198–207. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.003.Spontaneous
  • de Hevia, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Number-space mapping in human infants. Psychological Science, 21(5), 653–660. doi:10.1177/0956797610366091.Number-Space
  • de Hevia, M. D., Veggiotti, L., Streri, A., & Bonn, C. D. (2017). At birth, humans associate “few” with left and “many” with right. Current Biology, 27(24), 3879–3884.e2. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.024
  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122(3), 371–396. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
  • Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition, 116(2), 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007
  • Fischer, M. H. (2003). Cognitive representation of negative numbers. Psychological Science, 14(3), 278–282. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.03435
  • Fischer, M. H. (2012). A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition. Cognitive Processing, 13(1 SUPPL), 161–164. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0477-5
  • Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: Spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(OCT), 1–7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556. doi:10.1038/nn1066
  • Fischer, M. H., & Rottmann, J. (2005). Do negative numbers have a place on the mental number line? Psychology Science, 47(1), 22–32.
  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition — From single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.927515
  • Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.2.91
  • Fischer, U., Fischer, M. H., Huber, S., Strauß, S., & Moeller, K. (2018). The influence of number magnitude on continuous swiping movements. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4(2), 297–316. doi:10.5964/jnc.v4i2.135
  • Fischer, U., Link, T., Cress, U., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2015). Math with the dance mat: on the benefits of numerical training approaches. In V. R. Lee (Ed.), Learning technologies and the body: Integration and implementation in formal and informal learning environments (pp. 149–166). New York: Routledge.
  • Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 177–183. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3
  • Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Class, F., Huber, S., Cress, U., Fischer, U., & Nuerk, H. (2016). Dancing with the SNARC : Measuring spatial-numerical associations on a digital dance mat. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1037/cep0000084
  • Ganor-Stern, D., Pinhas, M., Kallai, A., & Tzelgov, J. (2010). Holistic representation of negative numbers is formed when needed for the task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1969–1981. doi:10.1080/17470211003721667
  • Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, 122(3), 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004
  • Gibson, L. C., & Maurer, D. (2016). Development of SNARC and distance effects and their relation to mathematical and visuospatial abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 301–313. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.009
  • Göbel, S. M., McCrink, K., Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2018). Observation of directional storybook reading influences young children’s counting direction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 49–66. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.001
  • Gullick, M. M., & Wolford, G. (2014). Brain systems involved in arithmetic with positive versus negative numbers. Human Brain Mapping, 35(2), 539–551. doi:10.1002/hbm.22201
  • Hartmann, M., Gashaj, V., Stahnke, A., & Mast, F. (2014). There is more than “more is up”: Hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1401–1414. doi:10.1037/a0036686
  • Hoffmann, D., Hornung, C., Martin, R., & Schiltz, C. (2013). Developing number-space associations: SNARC effects using a color discrimination task in 5-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 775–791. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.013
  • Huber, S., Bahnmueller, J., Klein, E., & Moeller, K. (2015). Testing a model of componential processing of multi-symbol numbers—Evidence from measurement units. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(5), 1417–1423. doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0805-8
  • Huber, S., Cornelsen, S., Moeller, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2014). Towards a model of generalized parallel componential processing of multi-symbol numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 732–745. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000043
  • Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 662–673. doi:10.3758/BF03195857
  • Krajcsi, A., & Igács, J. (2010). Processing negative numbers by transforming negatives to positive range and by sign shortcut. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(7), 1021–1038. doi:10.1080/09541440903211113
  • Kucian, K., Plangger, F., O’Gorman, R., & Von Aster, M. (2013). Operational momentum effect in children with and without developmental dyscalculia. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(NOV), 1–3. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00847
  • Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., & Brugger, P. (2008). Looking for the answer. The Mind’s Eye in Number Space, 151, 725–729. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.07.068
  • Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), R264–R265. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.015
  • Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General magnitude representation in human infants. Psychological Science, 21(6), 873–881. doi:10.1177/0956797610370158.General
  • Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Frith, C. D., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science, 280, 921–924. doi:10.1126/science.280.5365.921
  • Mann, A., Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H. C. (2012). On the development of Arabic three-digit number processing in primary school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 594–601. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.002
  • Mann, A., Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Attentional strategies in place-value integration. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 219(1), 42–49. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000045
  • Masson, M. E. J. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 679–690. doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0049-5
  • Mathieu, R., Epinat-Duclos, J., Léone, J., Fayol, M., Thevenot, C., & Prado, J. (2018). Hippocampal spatial mechanisms relate to the development of arithmetic symbol processing in children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(August 2016), 324–332. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.06.001
  • Mathieu, R., Epinat-Duclos, J., Sigovan, M., Breton, A., Cheylus, A., Fayol, M., … Prado, J. (2017). What’s behind a “+” sign? Perceiving an arithmetic operator recruits brain circuits for spatial orienting. Cerebral Cortex, (May), 1–12. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx064
  • McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 400–408. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.013
  • Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2009). Sequential or parallel decomposed processing of two-digit numbers? Evidence from eye-tracking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 323–334. doi:10.1080/17470210801946740
  • Moeller, K., Fischer, U., Link, T., Wasner, M., Huber, S., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H. C. (2012). Learning and development of embodied numerosity. Cognitive Processing, 13, 271–274. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0457-9
  • Müller, D., & Schwarz, W. (2007). Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 1151–1161. doi:10.3758/BF03193485
  • Ninaus, M., Moeller, K., Kaufmann, L., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H.-C., & Wood, G. (2017). Cognitive mechanisms underlying directional and non-directional spatial-numerical associations across the lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(AUG), 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01421
  • Nuerk, H., Bauer, F., Krummenacher, J., Heller, D., & Willmes, K. (2005). The power of the mental number line : How the magnitude of unattended numbers affects performance in an Eriksen task. Psychology Science, 47(1), 34–50.
  • Nuerk, H., & Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A(5), 835–863. doi:10.1080/02724980343000512
  • Nuerk, H., & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers Three models of two-digit magnitude representation. Psychology Science, 47(1), 52–72.
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Kaufmann, L., Zoppoth, S., & Willmes, K. (2004). On the development of the mental number line: More, less, or never holistic with increasing age? 40(6), 1199–1211. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1199
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Patro, K., Cress, U., Schild, U., Friedrich, C. K., & Göbel, S. M. (2015). How space-number associations may be created in preliterate children: Six distinct mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(MAR), 1–6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00215
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25–B33. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00142-1
  • Patro, K., Fischer, U., Nuerk, H.-C., & Cress, U. (2015). How to rapidly construct a spatial–Numerical representation in preliterate children (at least temporarily). Developmental Science, 19(1), 126–144. doi:10.1111/desc.12296
  • Patro, K., & Haman, M. (2012). The spatial–Numerical congruity effect in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 534–542. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.09.006
  • Patro, K., Nuerk, H. C., Cress, U., & Haman, M. (2014). How number-space relationships are assessed before formal schooling: A taxonomy proposal. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(MAY), 1–6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00419
  • Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition, 109(3), 408–415. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.003
  • Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: Operation signs have spatial associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1527–1540. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.892516
  • Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Whorf reloaded: Language effects on non-verbal number processing in 1st grade - a trilingual study. Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 371–382.
  • Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Random walks on the mental number line. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 43–49. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3718-7
  • Shaki, S., & Petrusic, W. M. (2005). On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 931–937. doi:10.3758/BF03196788
  • Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(2), 164–182. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164
  • van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access to number magnitude: A study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 99–113. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001
  • Varma, S., & Schwartz, D. L. (2011). The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts. Cognition, 121(3), 363–385. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.005
  • White, S. L. J., Szucs, D., & Soltész, F. (2012). Symbolic number: The integration of magnitude and spatial representations in children aged 6 to 8years. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(JAN), 1–11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00392
  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(4), 489–525.
  • Yang, T., Chen, C., Zhou, X., Xu, J., & Dong, Q. (2014). Development of spatial representation of numbers: A study of the SNARC effect in Chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.011
  • Zhou, X., Chen, Y., Chen, C., Jiang, T., Zhang, H., & Dong, Q. (2007). Chinese kindergartners ’ automatic processing of numerical magnitude in Stroop-like tasks. Memory & Cognition, 35(3), 464–470.
  • Zorzi, M., Bonato, M., Treccani, B., Scalambrin, G., & Marenzi, R. (2012). Neglect impairs explicit processing of the mental number line. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(May), 1–12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00125

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.