2,810
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

The Role of Non-symbolic and Symbolic Skills in the Development of Early Numerical Cognition from Preschool to Kindergarten Age

References

  • Alloway, T. P. (2007). Automated working memory assessment (Translated and reproduced by permission of Pearson Assessment). London, England: Pearson Assessment.
  • Brankaer, C., Ghesquière, P., & De Smedt, B. (2014). Children’s mapping between non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitudes and its association with timed and untimed tests of mathematics achievement. PLoS One, 9(4), e93565. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0093565
  • Cantrell, L., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Open questions and a proposal: A critical review of the evidence on infant numerical abilities. Cognition, 128(3), 331–352. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.008
  • Clearfield, M. W., & Mix, K. S. (2001). Amount versus number: Infants’ use of area and contour length to discriminate small sets. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(3), 243–260. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD0203_1
  • Darlington, R. B. (1968). Multiple regression in psychological research and practice. Psychological Bulletin, 69(3), 161–182. doi:10.1037/h0025471
  • Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1–2), 1–42. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N
  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83–120.
  • Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E., & Pica, P. (2008). Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science, 320, 1217–1220. doi:10.1126/science.1156540
  • Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123, 53–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013
  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
  • Friso-van den Bos, I., Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2014). Number sense in kindergarten children: Factor structure and working memory predictors. Learning and Individual Differences, 33, 23–29. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.003
  • JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.13.1) [Computer software] Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
  • Jiménez Lira, C., Carver, M., Douglas, H., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2017). The integration of symbolic and non-symbolic representations of exact quantity in preschool children. Cognition, 166, 382–397. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.033
  • Kim, D., & Opfer, J. (2017). A unified framework for bounded and unbounded numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 53(6), 1088–1097. doi:10.1037/dev0000305
  • Kim, D., & Opfer, J. (2018). Dynamics and development in number-to-space mapping. Cognitive Psychology, 107, 44–66. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.10.001
  • Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). Early numerical development and the role of non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction, 25, 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001
  • Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Early development of quantity to number-word linkage as a precursor of mathematical school achievement and mathematical difficulties: Findings from a four-year longitudinal study. Learning and Instruction, 19(6), 513–526. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.10.002
  • Kroesbergen, E. H., Van ‘T Noordende, J. E., & Kolkman, M. E. (2014). Training working memory in kindergarten children: Effects on working memory and early numeracy. Child Neuropsychology, 20(1), 23–37. doi:10.1080/09297049.2012.736483
  • Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Luit, J. E. H., Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M., Van Loosbroek, E., & Van de Rijt, B. A. M. (2009). Individual differences in early numeracy the role of executive functions and subitizing. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(3), 226–236. doi:10.1177/0734282908330586
  • Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105(2), 395–438. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005
  • Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Preschool children’s mapping of number words to nonsymbolic numerosities. Child Development, 76, 978–988. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00891.x
  • Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General magnitude representation in human infants. Psychological Science, 21(6), 873–881. doi:10.1177/0956797610370158
  • Ludlow, L., & Klein, K. (2014). Suppressor variables: The difference between ‘is’ versus ‘acting as’. Journal of Statistics Education, 22(2), 1–28. doi:10.1080/10691898.2014.11889703
  • McCrink, K., & Opfer, J. E. (2014). Development of spatial-numerical associations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(6), 439–445. doi:10.1177/0963721414549751
  • Núñez, R. E. (2017). Is there really an evolved capacity for number? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 409–424. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.005
  • Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., Defever, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 344–357. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x
  • Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 418–431. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012
  • Sasanguie, D., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Number comparison and number line estimation rely on different mechanisms. Psychologica Belgica, 53(4), 17–35. doi:10.5334/pb-53-4-17
  • Sasanguie, D., Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). Predictors for mathematics achievement? Evidence from a longitudinal study. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6(3), 119–128. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01147.x
  • Schneider, M., Merz, S., Stricker, J., De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L., & Luwel, K. (2018). Associations of number line estimation with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(5), 1467–1484. doi:10.1111/cdev.13068
  • Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14(3), 237–243. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02438
  • Soltész, F., Szűcs, D., & Szűcs, L. (2010). Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4-to 7-year-old children: A developmental study. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6(13), 1–14. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-13
  • Starr, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(45), 18116–18120. doi:10.1073/pnas.1302751110
  • Van ‘t Noordende, J. E., Volman, M. J. M., Leseman, P. P. M., Moeller, K., Dackermann, T., & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2018). The use of local and global ordering strategies in number line estimation in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1562), 1–16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01562
  • Van de Schoot, R., Kaplan, D., Denissen, J., Asendorpf, J. B., Neyer, F. J., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2014). A gentle introduction to Bayesian analyses: Applications to developmental research. Child Development, 85(3), 842–860. doi:10.1111/cdev.12169
  • Van Doorn, J., Van den Bergh, D., Bohm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., … Wagenmakers, E. (2019). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Preprint. doi:10.31234/osf.io/yqxfr
  • Van Luit, J. E. H., & Van de Rijt, B. A. M. (2008). Utrechtse Getalbegrip Toets-Revised [Early numeracy test-revised]. Doetinchem, the Netherlands: Graviant.
  • Wynn, K. (1990). Childrens understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155–193. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3