590
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Selective Trust and Theory of Mind in Brazilian Children: Effects of Socioeconomic Background

References

  • Araújo, M. V. M., Marteleto, M. R. F., & Schoen-Ferreira, T. H. (2010). Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares [Assessment of receptive vocabular in preschool children]. Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas), 27(2), 169–176. doi:10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004
  • Birch, S. A., Vauthier, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2008). Three-and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107(3), 1018–1034. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008
  • Brosseau‐Liard, P., Penney, D., & Poulin‐Dubois, D. (2015). Theory of mind selectively predicts preschoolers’ knowledge‐based selective word learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 464–475. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12107
  • Capovilla, F., Nunes, L., Nogueira, D., Nunes, D., Araújo, I., Bernart, A., & Capovilla, A. (1997). Desenvolvimento do vocabulário receptivo-auditivo da pré-escola à oitava série: Normatização fluminense baseada em aplicação coletiva da tradução brasileira do Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [The development of receptive hearing vocabulary from preschool to seventh grade: Norms based on the collective administration of the Brazilian translation of the peabody picture vocabulary test]. Ciência Cognitiva: Teoria, Pesquisa e Aplicação, 1, 381–440.
  • Capovilla, F. C., & Capovilla, A. G. S. (1997). Desenvolvimento lingüístico na criança dos dois aos seis anos: Tradução e estandardização do Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test de Dunn & Dunn, e da Language Deveopment Survey de Rescorla [Linguistic development in children from 2 to 6: Translation and standardization of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test by Dunn & Dunn and the Language Development Survey by Rescorla]. Ciência Cognitiva: Teoria, Pesquisa e Aplicação, 1, 353–380.
  • Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J. B., & Mercier, H. (2016). The influence of power and reason on young Maya children’s endorsement of testimony. Developmental Science, 19(6), 957–966. doi:10.1111/desc.12336
  • Corriveau, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12(3), 426–437. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x
  • Corriveau, K. H., Fusaro, M., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20(3), 372–377. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02291.x
  • Corriveau, K. H., Harris, P. L., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Elliott, L., … De Rosnay, M. (2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80(3), 750–761. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01295.x
  • Corriveau, K. H., Meints, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Early tracking of informant accuracy and inaccuracy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 331–342. doi:10.1348/026151008X310229
  • Del Vicario, M., Bessi, A., Zollo, F., Petroni, F., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., … Quattrociocchi, W. (2016). The spreading of misinformation online. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(3), 554–559. doi:10.1073/pnas.1517441113
  • Dessen, M. A., & Souza, D. H. (2014). Theory-of-mind development in Brazilian low-income children. International Education Research, 2(3), 41–52. doi:10.12735/ier.v2i3p41
  • Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: A meta-analysis.Child development,89(3), 971–987.
  • Dias, M. G. B. B. (1993). O desenvolvimento do conhecimento da criança sobre a mente [The development of children’s knowledge about the mind]. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 9, 587–600.
  • DiYanni, C., Nini, D., Rheel, W., & Livelli, A. (2012). ‘I won’t trust you if I think you’re trying to deceive me’: Relations between selective trust, theory of mind, and imitation in early childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(3), 354–371. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.590462
  • Dunfield, K. A., Kuhlmeier, V. A., & Murphy, L. (2013). Children’s use of communicative intent in the selection of cooperative partners. PloS One, 8(4), e61804. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061804
  • Ebert, S., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., & Weinert, S. (2017). Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 44, 32–48. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.005
  • Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 18(4), 741–770. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00077
  • Fujisawa, K. K., Kutsukake, N., & Hasegawa, T. (2008). Reciprocity of prosocial behavior in Japanese preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(2), 89–97. doi:10.1177/0165025407084055
  • Fusaro, M., & Harris, P. L. (2008). Children assess informant reliability using bystanders’ non‐verbal cues. Developmental Science, 11(5), 771–777. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00728.x
  • Harris, P. L., Koenig, M. A., Corriveau, K. H., & Jaswal, V. K. (2018). Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 251–273. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710
  • Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002
  • Jaswal, V. K. (2007). The effect of vocabulary size on toddlers’ receptiveness to unexpected testimony about category membership. Infancy, 12(2), 169–187. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00239.x
  • Kim, S., Paulus, M., Sodian, B., Itakura, S., Ueno, M., Senju, A., & Proust, J. (2018). Selective learning and teaching among Japanese and German children. Developmental Psychology, 54(3), 536. doi:10.1037/dev0000441
  • Koenig, M. A., & McMyler, B. (2019). Testimonial knowledge: Understanding the evidential, uncovering the interpersonal. In M. Fricker, P. Graham, D. Henderson, N. Pederson, & J. Wyatt (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of social epistemology(pp. 103-114). New York, NY: Routledge Publ.
  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76(6), 1261–1277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
  • Koenig, M. A., & Jaswal, V. K. (2011). Characterizing children’s expectations about expertise and incompetence: Halo or pitchfork effects? Child Development, 82(5), 1634–1647. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01618.x
  • Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 46(4), 815. doi:10.1037/a0019664
  • Kushnir, T., & Koenig, M. A. (2017). What I don’t know won’t hurt you: The relation between professed ignorance and later knowledge claims. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 826–835. doi:10.1037/dev0000294
  • Levine, D., Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., de Villiers, J., … Wilson, M. S. (2020). Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax and language process skills with the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 114–128. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.006
  • Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., & Cook, J. (2017). Beyond misinformation: Understanding and coping with the “post-truth” era. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 353–369. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.07.008
  • Li, X., & Yow, W. Q. (2018). Willingness to revise own testimony: 3- and 4-year-olds’ selective trust in unexpected testimony from accurate and inaccurate informants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.008
  • Lucas, A. J., Lewis, C., Pala, F. C., Wong, K., & Berridge, D. (2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers’ selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 579. doi:10.1037/a0029864
  • Mills, C. M., & Elashi, F. B. (2014). Children’s skepticism: Developmental and individual differences in children’s ability to detect and explain distorted claims. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 124, 1–17. doi:10.1016/J.JECP.2014.01.015
  • Moretti, T. C. D. F., Kuroishi, R. C. S., & Mandrá, P. P. (2017). Vocabulary of preschool children with typical language development and socioeducational variables. In CoDAS (Vol. 29, No. 1). doi:10.1590/2317-1782/20172016098
  • Moruzzi, A. B., & da Silva, B. N. B. (2017). A universalização da educação infantil nas universidades federais: Um estudo a partir dos editais de abertura de vagas nas unidades [The universalization of child education in federal universities: A study based on calls por openings in the educational units]. Revista Nupem, 9(16), 116–131. doi:10.33871/nupem.v9i16.141
  • O’Connor, C., & Weatherall, J. O. (2019). The misinformation age. New Haven & London: Yale University Press.
  • Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285–308. doi:10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226
  • Palmen, S. H. D. C. (2007). A constituição das creches nas universidades públicas estaduais paulistas e os direitos sociais da mulher e da criança pequena [The constitution of daycare centers in public universities from the state of São Paulo and women and young children’s social rights]. Proposições, 18(3), 231–244.
  • Pasquini, E. S., Corriveau, K. H., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2007). Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1216. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216
  • Reifen Tagar, M., Federico, C. M., Lyons, K. E., Ludeke, S., & Koenig, M. A. (2014). Heralding the authoritarian? Orientation toward authority in early childhood. Psychological Science, 25(4), 883–892. doi:10.1177/0956797613516470
  • Roazzi, A., & Santana, S. M. (1999). Teoria da mente: Efeito da idade, do sexo e do uso de atores animados e inanimados na inferência de estados mentais [Theory of mind: The effect of age, sex and the use of animate and inanimate “actors” in inferring mental states]. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 12, 1–17.
  • Rodrigues, M. C., Pelisson, M. C. C., Silveira, F. F., Ribeiro, N. N., & Silva, R. D. L. M. D. (2015). Evaluation of theory of mind: A study with students from public and private schools. Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas), 32(2), 213–220. doi:10.1590/0103-166X2015000200006
  • Ruffman, T., Taumoepeau, M., & Perkins, C. (2012). Statistical learning as a basis for social understanding in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 87–104. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02045.x
  • Silva, R. L. M. (2020). Teoria da mente, funções executivas e regulação emocional na infância: um estudo com alunos do ensino fundamental [Theory of mind, executive functions and emotional regulation in childhood: a study with school children] ( Unpublished doctoral dissertation). PPG Psi, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora.
  • Sobel, D. M., & Finiasz, Z. (2020). How children learn from others: An analysis of selective word learning. Child Development, 91(6), e1134–e1161. doi:10.1111/cdev.13415
  • Souza, D. H., & Messias, A. C. (2020, November 20). Selective trust in preschoolers: A systematic review. Psicologia em Estudo, 25, e44631. doi:10.4025/psicolestud.v25i0.44631
  • Stamos, A., Altsitsiadis, E., & Dewitte, S. (2019). Investigating the effect of childhood socioeconomic background on interpersonal trust: Lower childhood socioeconomic status predicts lower levels of trust. Personality and Individual Differences, 145, 19–25. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.03.011
  • Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic (pp. 392). Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511614712
  • Stephens, E., Suarez, S., & Koenig, M. (2015). Early testimonial learning: Monitoring speech acts and speakers. In Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 48, pp. 151–183). JAI. doi:10.1016/bs.acdb.2014.11.004
  • Tebet, G. D. C., & Abramowicz, A. (2010). Creches, educação infantil e políticas públicas municipais: Um olhar sobre a cidade de São Carlos-Brasil [Daycare centers, preschool education and municipal public policy: A study on the city of São Carlos-Brazil]. Políticas Educativas, 3(2), 25–39.
  • Tudge, J. R., Doucet, F., Odero, D., Sperb, T. M., Piccinini, C. A., & Lopes, R. S. (2006). A window into different cultural worlds: Young children’s everyday activities in the United States, Brazil, and Kenya. Child Development, 77(5), 1446–1469. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00947.x
  • Vahid, N., & Farshid, A. (2017). Development of trust in a sample of iranian children. Clinical Psychology Studies, 7(27), 71–88.
  • Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory‐of‐mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
  • Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18(2), 129–166. doi:10.1007/BF02249397

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.