3,288
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension account for SES-differences in how school-aged children infer word meanings from sentences

ORCID Icon, &

References

  • Abel, A. D., Schneider, J., & Maguire, M. J. (2018). N400 response indexes word learning from linguistic context in children. Language Learning and Development, 14(1), 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2017.1362347
  • Abel, A. D., Sharp, B. J., & Konja, C. (2020). Investigating implicit and explicit word learning in school-age children using a combined behavioral-Event Related Potential (ERP) approach Developmental Neuropsychology . 45(1), 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2019.1709465
  • Bornstein, R. F., Languirand, M. A., Geiselman, K. J., Creighton, J. A., West, M. A., Gallagher, H. A., & Eisenhart, E. A. (2003). Construct validity of the relationship profile test: A self-report measure of dependency-detachment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 64–74. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA8001_15
  • Burchinal, M., Foster, T. J., Bezdek, K. G., Bratsch-Hines, M., Blair, C., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2020). School-entry skills predicting school-age academic and social–emotional trajectories. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 51, 67–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.08.004
  • Cain, K., Lemmon, K., & Oakhill, J. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671–681. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.671
  • Campbell, T. F., Dollaghan, C. A., Rackette, H. E., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Shriberg, L. D., Sabo, D. L., & Kurs-Lasky, M. (2003). Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Development, 74(2), 346–357. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402002
  • Campbell, J. M., & Dommestrup, A. K. (2010). Peabody picture vocabulary test. In The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology (pp. 1). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0649
  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). The American Heritage word frequency book. Houghton Mifflin Boston.
  • Chiat, S., & Polišenská, K. (2016). A framework for crosslinguistic nonword repetition tests: Effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(5), 1179–1189. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0293
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
  • Coleman, J. S. (1966). Equality of educational opportunity. US Dept of Health, Education and Welfare. https://www.worldcat.org/title/equality-of-educational-opportunity/oclc/2806374
  • DeGarmo, D. S., Forgatch, M. S., & Martinez, C. R., Jr. (1999). Parenting of divorced mothers as a link between social status and boys’ academic outcomes: Unpacking the effects of socioeconomic status. Child Development, 70(5), 1231–1245. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00089
  • Dolean, D., Melby-Lervåg, M., Tincas, I., Damsa, C., & Lervåg, A. (2019). Achievement gap: Socioeconomic status affects reading development beyond language and cognition in children facing poverty. Learning and Instruction, 63, 101218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101218
  • Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 41(5), 1136–1146. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136
  • Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Janosky, J. E., Pitcairn, D. N., & Kurs-Lasky, M. (1999). Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1432–1443. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4206.1432
  • Elleman, A. M., Oslund, E. L., Griffin, N. M., & Myers, K. E. (2019). A review of middle school vocabulary interventions: Five research-based recommendations for practice. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(4), 477–492. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-VOIA-18-0145
  • Engel, P. M. J., Santos, F. H., & Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(6), 1580–1587. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0210)
  • Ensminger, M. E., & Fothergill, K. E. (2014). A decade of measuring SES: What it tells us and where to go from here. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development (pp. 13–27). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410607027-9
  • Evans, G. W., & Schamberg, M. A. (2009). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(16), 6545–6549. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811910106
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., Tomasello, M., Mervis, C. B., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), i–185. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166093
  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019
  • Frishkoff, G. A., Collins-Thompson, K., Perfetti, C. A., Callan, J., & Frishkoff, G. A. (2008). Measuring incremental changes in word knowledge: Experimental validation and implications for learning and assessment. Behavior Research Methods, 40(4), 907–925. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.4.907
  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., Nolan, D., & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: An effective aid to preschool children's learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(7), 991–1004.
  • Hall, A. H., & Tannebaum, R. P. (2013). Test review: J. L. Wiederholt & B. R. Bryant. (2012). Gray oral reading tests—fifth edition (GORT-5). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 31(5), 516–520. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282912468578
  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. P.H. Brookes.
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based (approach edn ed., p. 120). New York: Guilford Publications.
  • Hill, M. S., Wagovich, S. A., & Manfra, L. (2017). Word learning during reading: Effects of language ability in school-age children. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 39(1), 270–280. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740117702457
  • Hill, M. S., & Wagovich, S. A. (2020). Word learning from context in school-age children: Relations with language ability and executive function. Journal of Child Language, 47(5), 1006–1029. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000989
  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., Yust, P. K. S., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–1083. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493
  • Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612
  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
  • Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Bridges, K. (2011). Measurement and model building in studying the influence of socioeconomic status on child development. In L. Mayes & M. Lewis (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of environment in human development. Cambridge Univ Press 590–606 . https://www.academia.edu/download/43698941/Measurement_and_Model_Building_in_Studyi20160313-1679-14gyahw.pdf
  • Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-ses and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238
  • Hoffman, L. (2003). Methodological issues in studies of SES, parenting, and child development. In Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development 125–143
  • Jackson, M., Kiernan, K., & Mclanahan, S. (2017 Maternal education, changing family circumstances, and children’s skill development in the United States and UK.). The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social .Science 67 1 59–84 . https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716217729471
  • Larsen, J. A., & Nippold, M. A. (2007). Morphological analysis in school-age children: Dynamic assessment of a word learning strategy. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(3), 201–212. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/021)
  • Levine, D., Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Michnick Golinkoff, R., de Villiers, J., Iglesias, A., & Sweig Wilson, M. (2020). Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax and language process skills with the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 114–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.006
  • Luo, R., Pace, A., Levine, D., Iglesias, A., de Villiers, J., Golinkoff, R. M., Wilson, M. S., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2021). Home literacy environment and existing knowledge mediate the link between socioeconomic status and language learning skills in dual language learners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 55, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.10.007
  • Maguire, M. J., Schneider, J. M., Middleton, A. E., Ralph, Y., Lopez, M., Ackerman, R. A., & Abel, A. D. (2018). Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 679–695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.003
  • Masek, L. R., Ramirez, A. G., McMillan, B. T. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2021). Beyond counting words: A paradigm shift for the study of language acquisition. Child Development Perspectives, 15(4), 274–280. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12425
  • Molfese, V. J., Modglin, A., & Molfese, D. L. (2003). The role of environment in the development of reading skills: A longitudinal study of preschool and school-age measures. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 59–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194030360010701
  • Nagy, W., Townsend, D., Lesaux, N., & Schmitt, N. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91–108. https://doi.org/10.1002/RRQ.011
  • Pace, A., Alper, R., Burchinal, M. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Measuring success: Within and cross-domain predictors of academic and social trajectories in elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 112–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001
  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891.
  • Ralph, Y. K., Schneider, J. M., Abel, A. D., & Maguire, M. J. (2020). Using the N400 event-related potential to study word learning from context in children from low- and higher-socioeconomic status homes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 191, 104758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104758
  • Richels, C., Johnson, K, Walden, T.A., Conture, E.G. (2013). Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. Journal of Communication 46 4 361–374 .
  • Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951–953. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167025
  • Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x
  • Rowe, M. L., & Weisleder, A. (2020). Language development in context. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2(1), 201–223. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-042220-121816
  • RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC. http://www.rstudio.com/
  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Henderson, A. M. E. (2013). Preschoolers are selective word learners. Cognition. 126 2 , 177–180 doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.006.
  • Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7(4), 264–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1393
  • Shavlik, M., Davis‐Kean, P. E., Schwab, J. F., & Booth, A. E. (2020). Early word‐learning skills: A missing link in understanding the vocabulary gap? Developmental Science, 24(2). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13034
  • Spencer, E. J., & Schuele, C. M. (2012). An examination of fast mapping skills in preschool children from families with low socioeconomic status. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(10), 845–862. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2012.705215
  • Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, W. E. (2005). . In Teaching word meaning. Malwah: Routledge. https://www.scirp.org/(S(lz5mqp453edsnp55rrgjct55))/reference/ReferencesPapers.aspx?ReferenceID=2055031
  • Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1993). Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 211–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.2.211
  • Vittinghoff, E., Sen, Ś., & McCulloch, C. E. (2009). Sample size calculations for evaluating mediation. Statistics in Medicine, 28(4), 541–557.
  • Wagner, R. K., & Meros, D. (2010). Vocabulary and reading comprehension: Direct, indirect, and reciprocal influences. Focus on Exceptional Children, 2010. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24860194
  • Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145