583
Views
72
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill

&
Pages 1974-1991 | Received 25 Jan 2012, Published online: 08 Mar 2013

REFERENCES

  • Bakker, M., van Dijk, A., & Wicherts, J. M. (2012). The rules of the game called psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 543–554. doi: 10.1177/1745691612459060
  • Bull, R., Cleland, A. A., & Mitchell, T. (2012). Sex differences in the spatial representation of number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, , Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0028387
  • Cipora, K., Wood, G.. (2012, January). Optimal power to detect (between group differences) in SNARC–Monte Carlo study. Poster presented on XXXth European Workshop on Cognitive Neuropsychology, Bressanone, Italy
  • Dehaene, S. (2001). Precis of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16–36. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00154
  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
  • Ferguson, C. J., & Heene, M. (2012). A vast graveyard of undead theories: Publication bias and psychological science's aversion to the null. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 555–561. doi: 10.1177/1745691612459059
  • Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d'Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95–110. doi: 10.1080/135467996387552
  • Fias, W., van Dijck, J.-P., & Gevers, W. (2011). How number is associated with space? The role of working memory. In S. Dehaene & E. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought (pp. 133–148). LondonUK: Elsevier.doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-385948-8.00010-4
  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial–numerical associations. Cortex, 44, 386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.004
  • Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2010). How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial–numerical associations. Brain and Cognition, 72, 333–336. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010
  • Fischer, M. H., & Rottmann, J. (2005). Do negative numbers have a place on the mental number line? Psychology Science, 47, 22–32.
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503
  • Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2010). Verbal–spatial and visuospatial coding of number–space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 180–190. doi: 10.1037/a0017688
  • Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32
  • Göbel, S. M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: A review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 543–565. doi: 10.1177/0022022111406251
  • Hines, T. M. (1990). An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits. Memory & Cognition, 18, 40–46. doi: 10.3758/BF03202644
  • Hoffmann, D., Mussolin, C., & Schiltz, C. (2012). The interaction between number and space processing and math achievement in adults. Cognitive Processing, 13(1, Suppl. 1), 60
  • Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in Middle-Eastern and Western individuals: An online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 566–578. doi: 10.1177/0022022111406254
  • Lyons, J. (1969). Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Masson, M. E. J. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis testing. Behaviour Research Methods, 43, 679–690. doi: 10.3758/s13428-010-0049-5
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Iversen, W., & Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness association of response codes) effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: A, 57, 835–863.doi:10.1080/02724980343000512
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line—A review of multi-digit number processing. Journal of Psychology, 219, 3–22. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000041
  • Nuerk, H.-C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect. The association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52, 187–194. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187
  • Pashler, H., & Wagenmakers. E.-J. (Eds.). (2012) Special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence? [Special section]. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 7(6), 528–654. Retrieved from http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6.toc
  • Pesenti, M. (2005). Calculation abilities in expert calculators. In CampbellJ. I. D. (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 413–430). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Priftis, K., Zorzi, M., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umilta, C. (2006). Explicit versus implicit processing of representational space in neglect: Dissociations in accessing the mental number line. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 680–688. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.680
  • Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. In MarsdenP. V. (Eds.), Sociological methodology 1995 (pp. 111–196). Cambridge: Blackwell.
  • Schneider, M., Grabner, R. H., & Paetsch, J. (2009). Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement: Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 359–372. doi: 10.1037/a0013840
  • Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 328–331. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.328
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  • Thayer, R. E. (1986). Activation–Deactivation Adjective Check List: Current overview and structural analysis. Psychological Reports, 58, 607–614. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1986.58.2.607
  • van Dijck, J.-P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition, 119, 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50, 489–525.doi:10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187
  • Zorzi, M., Bonato, M., Treccani, B., Scalambrin, G., Marenzi, R., & Priftis, K. (2012). Neglect impairs explicit processing of the mental number line. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 125. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00125

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.